Skip to main content
/v1/supplement/title

‘Willpower’ is not enough: time for a new approach to public health policy to prevent obesity

Background

In most high-income countries, we eat too much. Looking around the world, it seems that obesity parallels economic development. Within countries, there is a more mixed picture, but there are few people who actively choose to become overweight. Indeed, weight gain has occurred against a strong cultural pressure to be slim, widespread knowledge of the harms of being overweight, and many people spending time and money actively trying to control their weight. The Health Survey for England suggests almost half the adult population are trying to lose weight at any one time [1]. Yet, despite this, there is a persistent belief among the public and policymakers that the solution is more education and urging people to make the right choices.

Public health prevention policies should not be confused with interventions to support meaningful weight loss treatment for people living with obesity. The latter is best achieved with individual support and specific weight loss programmes. But successful prevention of weight primary weight gain or secondary regain will also be supported by an environment which does not encourage overconsumption.

Creating a supportive food environment

How can we do this? Research shows clearly that we overvalue individual decision-making and underestimate the impact of our environment on our behaviour. Consider our study where a supermarket removed chocolate from the most prominent places in selected shops in the run-up to Easter, though the products were still available for sale elsewhere in the store [2]. Prior to the experiment, sales of chocolate in these stores and matched controls, where chocolate was promoted as usual, were similar. In the stores with less prominent positioning, people bought 12% more chocolate in the period before Easter than during the preceding period, while in the stores with (typical) layouts, they bought 31% more. In intervention stores, people put fewer calories in their baskets than control stores. Modern food purchasing environments are set up to maximise profit and not health.

Perhaps we could learn to be hyper-vigilant when shopping, but this requires a level of executive functioning (‘willpower’) that is more that we can reasonably be expected to mobilise at every moment of the day, especially when we are stressed or distracted. Food cues are embedded throughout our environment. Moreover, they prime our behaviour in ways far more subtle than we consciously recognise. In another experiment, children watched food or toy advertisements prior to a cartoon. Later, they were offered a choice of foods to eat. Compared with children who had watched TV without food adverts, children who had seen food advertisements ate more [3]. The same was true when children saw a celebrity on TV who was associated with advertisements for crisps, even though no food was shown [4]. It is unlikely that people perceive that their ‘choice’ in these and other similar experiments had been shaped by the environment. So why does this happen?

The UK Government Foresight report on obesity in 2007 described a reinforcing loop where biological hunger signals dominate over the much weaker satiety cues [5]. What evolved as a survival strategy now leaves us vulnerable to an environment where food is palatable, available, and heavily marketed. Weight gain is an almost inevitable consequence in economically advantaged countries, yet we berate ourselves for lack of willpower. More importantly, our society, expressed through the action of our policymakers, continues to believe that individuals have more control over their choices than is actually the case. This thinking shapes the policy discourse and presents a challenge to the introduction of policies seen as curbing the ‘free market’.

Accepting the need to change our food environment is crucial to making progress towards societies with a healthier weight. This is not something individuals can do alone. In the mid-twentieth century, the food industry worked to provide more food to more people more cheaply following a period when the main threat was undernutrition, but the market needs a reset if it is to deliver for today’s health needs. That probably requires government intervention to encourage and support progressive businesses through a time of change. The soft drink industry levy in the UK provides a good example of what can be achieved. By incentivising reformulation of soft drinks, sugar intake from drinks fell by 30% without decreasing sales [6]. This small change to the environment is predicted to decrease the prevalence of obesity by 0.2–0.9% and the incidence of type 2 diabetes by 0.8–4.4/1000 person-years [7].

Just as no single change in the environment created the high prevalence of obesity, so no one policy can reverse that change. We need to accumulate policies, as we have done in tobacco control, to reverse the environmental changes that have led to overconsumption. This requires sustained action, outlasting the usual political cycles.

But at present, standing between us and a healthier environment is policy inertia. We posit that our strong belief, arising from our daily experience of our self-conscious selves, leads us to consider that our behaviour is consciously governed because we do not perceive the myriad times a day when the environment changes what we do. While we can accept the intellectual argument that advertising works, we tend to view the effects of advertising as much greater on others rather than ourselves [8], and thus, our belief about the conscious drivers of our own behaviour remains intact. Moreover, as citizens or as policymakers, we have a strong moral belief that behaviour change should come from within and that external factors are somehow second-rate ways to change behaviour [9].

Conclusions

We have strong evidence that fiscal policies, advertising restrictions, and curtailing the availability of unhealthy products changes behaviour [10] and no shortage of policy documents recommending specific interventions to prevent obesity. Yet, only a few are enacted anywhere in the world. Explaining the neurobiological basis of behaviour does not seem to change our view that we are masters of our own destiny but highlighting the everyday experiences when our food ‘choices’ are shaped by the environment may be more persuasive in explaining why the ‘willpower’ model is flawed and, accordingly, open the door to more effective policy action.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

References

  1. Piernas C, Aveyard P, Jebb SA. Recent trends in weight loss attempts: repeated cross-sectional analyses from the health survey for England. Int J Obes. 2016;40(11):1754–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Piernas C, Harmer G, Jebb SA. Removing seasonal confectionery from prominent store locations and purchasing behaviour within a major UK supermarket: evaluation of a nonrandomised controlled intervention study. PLoS Med. 2022;19(3):e1003951.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Halford JCG, Gillespie J, Brown V, Pontin EE, Dovey TM. Effect of television advertisements for foods on food consumption in children. Appetite. 2004;42(2):221–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Boyland EJ, Harrold JA, Dovey TM, Allison M, Dobson S, Jacobs M-C, et al. Food choice and overconsumption: effect of a premium sports celebrity endorser. J Pediatr. 2013;163(2):339–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Butland B, Jebb S, Kopelman P, McPherson K, Thomas S, Mardell J, et al. Tackling obesities: future choices. Project report. London: Government Office for Science; 2007.

  6. Bandy LK, Scarborough P, Harrington RA, Rayner M, Jebb SA. Reductions in sugar sales from soft drinks in the UK from 2015 to 2018. BMC Med. 2020;18(1):20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Pell D, Mytton O, Penney TL, Briggs A, Cummins S, Penn-Jones C, et al. Changes in soft drinks purchased by British households associated with the UK soft drinks industry levy: controlled interrupted time series analysis. BMJ. 2021;372:n254.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Eisend M. The third-person effect in advertising: a meta-analysis. J Advert. 2017;46(3):377–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Morphett K, Partridge B, Gartner C, Carter A, Hall W. Why don’t smokers want help to quit? A qualitative study of smokers' attitudes towards assisted vs. unassisted quitting. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2015;12(6):6591–607.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Marteau TM, White M, Rutter H, Petticrew M, Mytton OT, McGowan JG, et al. Increasing healthy life expectancy equitably in England by 5 years by 2035: could it be achieved? Lancet. 2019;393(10191):2571–3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

SAJ is funded by the National Institute of Health Research (NIHR) Oxford University Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre and PA by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre. Both are funded by the Oxford and Thames Valley Applied Research Collaboration. PA is an NIHR Senior Investigator.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Both authors contributed equally. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Susan A. Jebb.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

SAJ is a member of BMC Medicine’s Editorial Board. The authors declare that they have no other competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jebb, S.A., Aveyard, P. ‘Willpower’ is not enough: time for a new approach to public health policy to prevent obesity. BMC Med 21, 89 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-023-02803-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-023-02803-z

Keywords