Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Ageing as risk factor for tinnitus and its complex interplay with hearing loss—evidence from online and NHANES data

Abstract

Background

Tinnitus affects 10 to 15% of the population, but its underlying causes are not yet fully understood. Hearing loss has been established as the most important risk factor. Ageing is also known to accompany increased prevalence; however, the risk is normally seen in context with (age-related) hearing loss. Whether ageing per se is a risk factor has not yet been established. We specifically focused on the effect of ageing and the relationship between age, hearing loss, and tinnitus.

Methods

We used two samples for our analyses. The first, exploratory analyses comprised 2249 Austrian individuals. The second included data from 16,008 people, drawn from a publicly available dataset (NHANES). We used logistic regressions to investigate the effect of age on tinnitus.

Results

In both samples, ageing per se was found to be a significant predictor of tinnitus. In the more decisive NHANES sample, there was an additional interaction effect between age and hearing loss. Odds ratio analyses show that per unit increase of hearing loss, the odds of reporting tinnitus is higher in older people (1.06 vs 1.03).

Conclusions

Expanding previous findings of hearing loss as the main risk factor for tinnitus, we established ageing as a risk factor in its own right. Underlying mechanisms remain unclear, and this work calls for urgent research efforts to link biological ageing processes, hearing loss, and tinnitus. We therefore suggest a novel working hypothesis that integrates these aspects from an ageing brain viewpoint.

Peer Review reports

Background

Tinnitus describes the conscious perception of a sound, such as a ringing or a hissing, in the absence of any identifiable external physical source. Most individuals will have experienced this phenomenon in a transient form at some point in their lives (e.g., following a concert). Estimates of prevalence vary, with ~ 15% of the adult population having perceived tinnitus at least occasionally [1], while the condition is chronic in approximately 10% of the adult population [2]. The extent to which tinnitus is experienced as bothersome varies, with a prevalence of approximately 1–3% of the adult population reporting at least moderate distress [2, 3]. Thus, beyond the individual level, tinnitus creates a significant financial burden on society, i.e., in terms of healthcare costs, missed days at work or early retirement [4]. Even though there are interventions to treat tinnitus-related distress [5], there is currently no clinical intervention that reliably eliminates the experience of the phantom sound. This deficit on the treatment side is paralleled by an absence of reliable methods (e.g., biomarkers) to diagnose tinnitus that go beyond subjective reports. This overall unsatisfactory state is ultimately due to an insufficient mechanistic understanding of why tinnitus develops and how it is manifested in changes to brain structure and function.

Among risk factors known to be associated with tinnitus, damage to hearing plays an outsized role. Animal models show that auditory deprivation due to hearing damage causes maladaptive changes in central auditory processing areas, such as increased activity, both spontaneous and synchronised [6, 7], that is “interpreted” as sound by downstream brain regions. In humans, 75–80% of individuals with unilateral or bilateral tinnitus are also diagnosed with hearing loss using standard pure-tone audiometry [8]. This, however, is likely a conservative lower bound, as hearing damage does not necessarily go along with audiometric hearing loss [9], which is also evident in individuals with tinnitus [10, 11]. Apart from the very mixed findings in humans in support of the hyperactivity/synchrony view [12], which is also clearly related to the challenge of operationalising putative neural processes using non-invasive methods such as fMRI or M/EEG, there is a clear explanatory gap with regard to the basic assumption that a majority of individuals with hearing loss do not develop tinnitus [13, 14]. Thus, while hearing loss is clearly important, there must be other factors that make some individuals more vulnerable to the development of tinnitus (see, e.g., our recent work on prediction engagement [15]).

Chronological age has also been associated with the prevalence of tinnitus [1,2,3] (for a negative finding however, see Oosterloo and colleagues [16]). Despite the statistical relationship, the process of ageing is usually not seen as a risk factor on its own, but rather simply related to the deterioration of hearing that goes along with getting older (i.e., age-related hearing loss; presbycusis) [17]. However, circumstantial evidence shows that ageing processes might constitute risk factors in their own right. For example, the greatest increase in tinnitus onset is around middle age (< 60 years), slightly prior to the greatest increase in the onset of moderate to strong hearing loss [18]. Furthermore, a recent retrospective study shows that tinnitus individuals were 63% more likely to develop early-onset dementia, while controlling for “confounding factors”, including hearing loss [19].

The aim of the present study is to shed new light on the relationship between (chronological) age, hearing loss, and tinnitus. In the first exploratory step, we used data from our ongoing online study Wie hört Salzburg? [20]. We then pooled publicly available data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) [21]. Using logistic regressions to predict the presence of tinnitus, we sought to establish ageing as a risk factor. The NHANES data indicated that ageing could make individuals more vulnerable to developing tinnitus when hearing loss is present. Our study underscores the importance of understanding the biological ageing processes that predispose people to tinnitus. A working hypothesis for future studies that synthesises tinnitus with research on hearing loss and cognitive decline will be outlined in the discussion.

Methods

Study 1

Design and participants

We initially focused on our Wie hört Salzburg? online study of hearing epidemiology in the county of Salzburg (Austria). The study included demographic information as well as questionnaires about tinnitus (German short version of Tinnitus Questionnaire, Mini-TQ [22]) and hearing characteristics (German version of the Speech, Spatial, and Qualities of Hearing Scale (SSQ) [23]). An online hearing test (Shoebox, Ottawa, Canada) was as well provided in the German language. In order to control for the impact of attention and environmental influences, we aimed to keep the duration of the online study short (~ 10–15 min) and explicitly instructed the participants to complete the study in a quiet setting without disruptions. Participants were instructed to perform the hearing test without any hearing aids but with headphones. Warble tone pulses were presented at our different frequencies (1000, 2000, 4000, and 6000 Hz), with increasing levels (dB hearing level), and participants were instructed to indicate the lowest threshold of subjective sound perception. These thresholds resulted in the mean hearing loss of the participants. To control for the accuracy of this online hearing test, we compared the results of several participants (n = 93) to their pure tone audiometry results conducted in our lab using Spearman’s rank correlation. Medium to strong correlations over the four frequencies supported the utility of the online hearing test as a screening tool (see Supplementary). We advertised the study via social media, at the university, and by directly contacting older people. The study is still ongoing, and we did not calculate the sample size in advance. Since tinnitus affects solely a relatively small portion of the population, we aimed for the largest possible sample size in the time period of around 1 year to ensure a sufficient number of participants with tinnitus in the dataset. We included participants aged 18–80 and excluded individuals with missing information. With this sample, we aimed first for an evaluation of the question whether age per se is a risk factor for tinnitus development.

Definition of variables

For our analyses, we focused on variables assessing the hearing abilities and tinnitus conditions of individual participants. The online hearing test yielded a hearing ability score in percentage, which was computed automatically by the provider. Tinnitus was assessed by asking whether participants ever experienced ringing in the ears for a longer period of time. Furthermore, tinnitus distress was investigated using the Mini-TQ.

Limitations

With respect to the study question, this sample has several limitations. First, age was not uniformly distributed, with fewer participants in the 30–40 age range. Second, the hearing test that we used was a commercial product sold as a screening tool. The results are correlated with the outcomes of pure-tone audiometry but are not suitable as a substitute. Third, we did not differentiate between past, acute, or chronic tinnitus in the questionnaire which limited the interpretability of the results regarding the effect of age on tinnitus. However, the distribution of hearing problems in our sample matched the results of the World Report on Hearing relatively well [17]. Due to the aforementioned limitations, we therefore used the Wie hört Salzburg? [20] dataset for exploratory analyses on the effect of ageing that motivated the analysis of an additional dataset to validate our findings.

Study 2

Design and participants

For a more representative sample, we included publicly available data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) [21]. Since 1999, the NHANES study has consisted of annual measurements, with approximately 5000 participants added to the database each year. Along with several other types of data, audiometric information was gathered. Individuals had objective pure-tone audiometry and were asked about the presence of tinnitus, the frequency of its occurrence, and the level of distress it caused. We merged data from the years 2001 to 2017, including participants up to age 75 with either no tinnitus at all or with chronic tinnitus, which resulted in a total sample size of 16,008 individuals.

Definition of variables

Hearing was objectively measured via pure-tone air conduction audiometry, including seven frequencies from 500 Hz to 8 kHz. A calibrated sound booth (manufactured by Acoustic Systems, model Delta 142) was used for the measurements conducted by highly trained health technologists (for more details on the audiometric procedure, see https://wwwn.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nhanes/2017-2018/manuals/2018-Audiometry-Procedures-Manual-508.pdf). For our analyses, we calculated the individual mean hearing ability, averaged across both ears, based on 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz, which is a common approach for averaging results of pure-tone audiometry (i.e., PTA-4, see for example Lin and colleagues, or Ozdek and colleagues [24, 25]). Tinnitus occurrence was investigated with the question, “In the past 12 months, have you ever had ringing, roaring, or buzzing in your ears?”. In the case of an affirmative response, participants were asked, “How often did this happen? Would you say…”. Potential answers were “almost always”, “at least once a day”, “at least once a week”, “at least once a month”, and “less frequently than once a month”. For our analyses, we included only participants with either no tinnitus or with tinnitus that occurred “almost always”. Therefore, we targeted chronic tinnitus alone and aimed for the greatest dissimilarity between groups in terms of experiencing tinnitus, rather than distinguishing acute or occasional tinnitus as well.

Statistical analysis

For both analyses, we used logistic regressions (family = binomial) implemented in R [26]. The models aimed to predict tinnitus based on the age and the mean hearing ability of both ears. For a more comprehensive view on the effect of ageing on tinnitus, we built two different regression models. In the first, simple model, we aimed to predict tinnitus solely based on age and hearing loss. In the second model, we included an additional interaction term to depict potential relations between age and hearing loss. In the last step, we compared the two models using a likelihood ratio test to quantify whether the improved fit of the more complex model is significantly better as compared to the more parsimonious model.

The age variable represented the age of the participants at the time the hearing test was conducted. Age was included in the model as a categorical variable. We therefore split the data by median into two groups, resulting in a cut-off age of 55 years in the first sample and a cut-off age of 34 years in the NHANES sample. In addition, we calculated the odds ratio for the NHANES sample.

Results

Our first sample consisted of 2249 inhabitants of Salzburg county (1147 female, 4 other, median = 55 years, mad = 13.34). We split the participants by median age into two groups, resulting in a mean age of 36.6 years (sd = 12.9 years) in the younger group and a mean age of 63.1 years (sd = 5.7 years) in the older group. A total of 695 participants indicated that they have experienced tinnitus. Tinnitus was detectable throughout all ages, and age was positively correlated (r(2247) = 0.499, p < 0.001) with mean hearing loss using Spearman’s rank correlation (Fig. 1a). Further analyses using logistic regressions emphasised hearing loss as a significant predictor of tinnitus development (b = 0.017, p < 0.001). However, older age was also a significant predictor (b =  − 0.538, p < 0.001) for tinnitus development—regardless of the extent of hearing loss. A second model, testing a potential interaction between age and hearing loss, yielded no significant effect (b =  − 0.004, p = 0.403) (Fig. 1b). These first, exploratory analyses suggested that age per se could be a risk factor for tinnitus. To follow up on these findings in more detail, we used the same methods on the more representative sample of the NHANES study.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Descriptives and logistic regression model of the Salzburg dataset. A Distribution of tinnitus over age and mean hearing loss. B Age characterisation was based on the median age of 55 years. The results showed enhanced tinnitus probability in older individuals compared to younger people over mean hearing loss scores

In this second sample, we included 16,008 individuals with an age range between 12 and 75 years (8044 females, median = 34 years, mad = 25.20). A median split by age resulted in a mean age of 19.9 years (sd = 6.08 years) in the younger group and a mean age of 53.6 years (sd = 11.9 years) in the older group. A total of 1017 participants experienced chronic tinnitus. As Fig. 2a indicates, the sample is more evenly distributed over age, and age was again correlated with hearing loss using Spearman’s rank correlation (r(16,006) = 0.621, p < 0.001). For a descriptive depiction of the sample, we computed the proportion of tinnitus and hearing loss over age groups (Fig. 2b). Hearing loss was defined as an average hearing ability over 25 dB. Tinnitus and hearing loss both increase with age, but to different extents. Whereas hearing loss rises rapidly with age, tinnitus peaks in the youngest age group, and then drops between ages 25 and 45. In the older age groups, the tinnitus proportion rises again, but to a much lesser extent than hearing loss. This also highlights that while many elderly people have hearing loss, not all of them also have tinnitus.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Descriptives of the NHANES dataset. A Distribution of tinnitus over age and mean hearing threshold. B Proportion of tinnitus and hearing loss in different age groups

In a simple regression model without interaction term, hearing loss was identified as a significant predictor of tinnitus (b = 0.057, p < 0.001), whereas age was not significant (b = 0.058, p = 0.469). However, a second model including a potential interaction term yielded significant effects for hearing loss (b = 0.061, p < 0.001) and age (b = 0.352, p = 0.001), as well as an interaction effect between the two predictors (b =  − 0.027, p < 0.001). Adding sex as a variable in both the simple (b =  − 0.027, p = 0.705) and the complex interaction model (b =  − 0.016, p = 0.820) did not show any influence. Therefore, we did not further include sex in the final models. Comparing the two models using a likelihood ratio test showed that the more complex interaction model was more superior in capturing the data as compared to the simpler model (deviance = 15.1, p < 0.001). The interaction effect suggests that hearing loss predicts the probability to report tinnitus, however, that this relationship is different for the two age groups. Indeed, Fig. 3 clearly shows that the probability to report tinnitus is similar for younger and older individuals when hearing loss is weak. However, the difference between the groups grows as hearing loss becomes more pronounced, with older individuals being more likely to report tinnitus.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Logistic regression model of the NHANES dataset. Age characterisation was based on the median age of 34 years. The results showed enhanced tinnitus probability in older individuals compared to younger people over mean hearing loss scores

To follow up this interaction in more quantitative terms, we calculated the odds ratio based on the regression coefficients of the second model. For an increase of one unit in hearing loss (here dB of the pure-tone audiometry), the odds ratio to report for younger people is 1.03 and for older people 1.06. This means, increasing hearing loss by one decibel, enhances the odds of tinnitus by 3% in younger people, whereas the odds for older people increases by 6%. Therefore, for the same amount of hearing loss increase, the odds of older people to report tinnitus are much higher as compared to younger individuals.

Discussion

The increased prevalence of tinnitus in older age is mostly interpreted in the context of hearing loss (presbycusis), for which mechanistic models also exist that are supported by animal studies. The main motivation of the current study was to understand whether ageing itself poses a risk factor for developing tinnitus. For this purpose, we used two separate study samples to quantify and statistically model the relationship between (chronological) age, hearing loss, and tinnitus. The first, exploratory analyses, using an online study of the local Salzburg population, suggested that age per se acts as a risk factor for tinnitus, even when controlling for hearing status. Critically, the NHANES data also provides more precise pure-tone audiometry data. Using this better-controlled data set, we were able to find an interaction effect between age and hearing loss. Sex, however, did not influence the effect. The interaction suggests that the risk to report tinnitus increases with hearing loss, but different for the age groups: estimation of odds ratio clearly showed much higher values in the older as compared to the younger group per unit increase of hearing loss (1.06 vs. 1.03 per dB mean hearing loss increase). Given the diverse nature of the two studies—each with its own purpose—the corroborating evidence makes a strong case for viewing ageing (or some other latent biological processes) as a tinnitus risk factor. This work strongly expands previous findings that focused on hearing loss as a major risk factor for developing tinnitus [8, 10]. Our findings suggest that current models need to be complemented to explain the impact of (chronological) age and hearing loss on developing tinnitus. This likely requires great research efforts into biological ageing processes that make individuals more vulnerable to developing tinnitus.

Strengths and limitations

A strength of the study design was the inclusion of a highly representative sample in addition to the study we conducted. In the NHANES study, tinnitus was measured using highly differentiating questions to detect chronic tinnitus. Additionally, hearing ability was assessed using the “gold standard” of pure-tone audiometry. Another strength of the study was the comparison of two different samples, showing the same main effects, despite major differences in the two datasets. Interpretation of the Austrian dataset was limited but strengthened the effects, since results derived from it showed the same direction as the NHANES study. It is further to be mentioned that the NHANES study is limited regarding confounding variables such as loud working environment, brain injuries, age at tinnitus onset or ancestral characteristics. These variables possibly affect tinnitus [27] and should be taken into account as well in future work with more comprehensive datasets. Overall, this data cannot give insights into the exact underlying ageing mechanisms that drive our findings. The present study, however, opens new avenues to guide more mechanistically oriented research and to promote urgently needed conceptual advancements in understanding tinnitus.

Future directions: towards an integrated understanding of tinnitus, hearing loss, and biological ageing

Altogether, our data provides strong support that ageing per se is a risk factor for tinnitus. However, the underlying mechanisms remain unclear. In order for the field to progress, the current study emphasises the necessity of a different line of tinnitus research that seriously considers biological ageing processes and integrates them into existing models. One direction could be to focus on age-related changes in the cochlea [12]. Another line of research could build upon the established relationship between hearing loss and cognitive decline [24]. While the exact mechanisms are not known [28], neural degeneration has been reported in individuals with hearing loss, especially in the hippocampus [29]. Thus, we suggest that hearing damage promotes the acceleration of brain ageing and that this is the actual factor that makes tinnitus more likely [30]. This working hypothesis generates several tangible predictions. For example, it would predict a statistical relationship between tinnitus and cognitive decline, a claim for which some tentative evidence exists [31]. Longitudinally, individuals with advanced brain age should be more prone to developing tinnitus. We suggest analysing differences in brain age primarily in a middle-aged group since we expect individual differences to be most distinguishable in middle age. In middle age, we expect differences in brain ages to be particularly influential on the future development of diseases or cognitive impairment. On the other hand, at an older age, relationships could be strongly blurred by other diseases and increasing mortality (life expectancy in the USA varies between 73 and 85 years) [32]. Another, perhaps more provocative, prediction would be that tinnitus itself is an indicator of advanced or accelerated brain ageing. Conducting large-scale studies using established MRI- or electrophysiology-based measures of brain age would be invaluable in pursuing this hypothesis [30, 33].

Conclusions

Overall, our results are striking testimony in support of recognising ageing per se as a tinnitus risk factor—rather than solely as an “enabler” of hearing loss, as previously assumed. Since chronological age, as used in the present study, is only a proxy for some latent causes, our work calls for urgent research efforts to be dedicated to linking biological ageing processes, hearing loss, and tinnitus. A conceptual integration is necessary, and we have advanced a novel working hypothesis that integrates the various interrelated domains (i.e., hearing loss—tinnitus, hearing loss—neural/cognitive decline, ageing—tinnitus) from a brain ageing viewpoint. Overall, we aim to motivate a series of studies that could significantly advance our understanding and therefore promote innovative treatment and prevention approaches.

Availability of data and materials

The National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey is a public open database. Data from our online study Wie hört Salzburg are available to any researcher. The corresponding author of the present study can be contacted to request the data used in the present analysis.

Abbreviations

NHANES:

National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey

References

  1. Biswas R, Lugo A, Akeroyd MA, Schlee W, Gallus S, Hall DA. Tinnitus prevalence in Europe: a multi-country cross-sectional population study. Lancet Reg Health Eur. 2022;12:100250.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Jarach CM, Lugo A, Scala M, et al. Global prevalence and incidence of tinnitus: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Neurol. 2022;79(9):888–900.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Baguley D, McFerran D, Hall D. Tinnitus. Lancet. 2013;382(9904):1600–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Trochidis I, Lugo A, Borroni E, et al. Systematic review on healthcare and societal costs of tinnitus. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(13):6881.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Hesser H, Weise C, Westin VZ, Andersson G. A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials of cognitive–behavioral therapy for tinnitus distress. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31(4):545–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Eggermont JJ, Roberts LE. The neuroscience of tinnitus. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27(11):676–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Schaette R. Tinnitus in men, mice (as well as other rodents), and machines. Hear Res. 2014;311:63–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Wallhäusser-Franke E, D’Amelio R, Glauner A, et al. Transition from acute to chronic tinnitus: predictors for the development of chronic distressing tinnitus. Front Neurol. 2017;8:605.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Liberman MC. Hidden hearing loss. Sci Am. 2015;313(2):48–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Schaette R, McAlpine D. Tinnitus with a normal audiogram: physiological evidence for hidden hearing loss and computational model. J Neurosci. 2011;31(38):13452–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Weisz N, Hartmann T, Dohrmann K, Schlee W, Norena A. High-frequency tinnitus without hearing loss does not mean absence of deafferentation. Hear Res. 2006;222(1–2):108–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Eggermont JJ, Roberts LE. Tinnitus: animal models and findings in humans. Cell Tissue Res. 2015;361(1):311–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Lockwood AH, Salvi RJ, Burkard RF. Tinnitus. N Engl J Med. 2002;347(12):904–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Tan CM, Lecluyse W, McFerran D, Meddis R. Tinnitus and patterns of hearing loss. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2013;14(2):275–82.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Partyka M, Demarchi G, Roesch S, et al. Phantom auditory perception (tinnitus) is characterised by stronger anticipatory auditory predictions. BioRxiv [Preprint]. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1101/869842. Accesed 6 July 2023.

  16. Oosterloo BC, Croll PH, Baatenburg de Jong RJ, Ikram MK, Goedegebure A. Prevalence of tinnitus in an aging population and its relation to age and hearing loss. J Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2021;164(4):859–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Highlighting priorities for ear and hearing care - World report on hearing. World Health Organization. 2021. https://www.who.int/activities/highlighting-priorities-for-ear-and-hearing-care/. Accessed 6 July 2023.

  18. Al-Swiahb J, Park SN. Characterization of tinnitus in different age groups: a retrospective review. Noise Health. 2016;18(83):214.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Cheng YF, Xirasagar S, Yang TH, Wu CS, Kao YW, Lin HC. Risk of early-onset dementia among persons with tinnitus: a retrospective case–control study. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Wie hört Salzburg?. https://salzburghoert.sbg.ac.at/. Accessed 6 July 2023.

  21. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. National Center for Health Statistics. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhanes/index.htm. Accessed 6 July 2023.

  22. Goebel G, Hiller W. Psychische Beschwerden bei chronischem Tinnitus: Erprobung und Bewertung des Tinnitus-Fragebogens (TF). Verhaltenstherapie. 1992;2(1):13–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Kiessling J, Grugel L, Meister H, Meis M. Übertragung der Fragebögen SADL, ECHO und SSQ ins Deutsche und deren Evaluation. Z Audio. 2011;50:6–16.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Lin FR, Metter EJ, O’Brien RJ, Resnick SM, Zonderman AB, Ferrucci L. Hearing loss and incident dementia. Arch Neurol. 2011;68(2):214–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Ozdek A, Karacay M, Saylam G, Tatar E, Aygener N, Korkmaz MH. Comparison of pure tone audiometry and auditory steady-state responses in subjects with normal hearing and hearing loss. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2010;267:43–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development Environment for R [Internet]. Boston, MA; 2015. Available from: http://www.rstudio.com/.

  27. Nondahl DM, Cruickshanks KJ, Huang GH, et al. Tinnitus and its risk factors in the Beaver Dam Offspring Study. Int J Audiol. 2011;50(5):313–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Griffiths TD, Lad M, Kumar S, et al. How can hearing loss cause dementia? Neuron. 2020;108(3):401–12.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Xu W, Zhang C, Li JQ, et al. Age-related hearing loss accelerates cerebrospinal fluid tau levels and brain atrophy: a longitudinal study. Aging (Albany NY). 2019;11(10):3156.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Cole JH, Marioni RE, Harris SE, Deary IJ. Brain age and other bodily ‘ages’: implications for neuropsychiatry. Mol psychiatry. 2019;24(2):266–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Jafari Z, Kolb BE, Mohajerani MH. Age-related hearing loss and tinnitus, dementia risk, and auditory amplification outcomes. Ageing Res Rev. 2019;56:100963.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Dwyer-Lindgren L, Kendrick P, Kelly YO, et al. Life expectancy by county, race, and ethnicity in the USA, 2000–19: a systematic analysis of health disparities. Lancet. 2022;400(10345):25–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Engemann DA, Mellot A, Höchenberger R, et al. A reusable benchmark of brain-age prediction from M/EEG resting-state signals. Neuroimage. 2022;262:119521.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

LR was supported by the Austrian Research Promotion Agency (FFG; BRIDGE 1 project “SmartCIs”, 871232) and Land Salzburg (“Hidden Hearing Loss”, 20204-WISS/225/288/4-2021).

Funding

The project was funded by the Austrian Research Promotion Agency (FFG; BRIDGE 1 project “SmartCIs”, 871232) and Land Salzburg (“Hidden Hearing Loss”, 20204-WISS/225/288/4–2021).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

LR, FS, and KB conceptualised, designed, acquired participants, and conducted the online study. LR and NW conducted the data analyses and drafted the manuscript. LR and NW directly accessed and verified the underlying data reported in the manuscript. LR, FS, KB, LV, SR, MK, and NW contributed to the interpretation of the results, revised the manuscript, and had final responsibility for the decision to submit the manuscript for publication. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lisa Reisinger.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

We obtained approval for the experimental procedure from the ethics committee of the University of Salzburg (EK-GZ: 22/2016 with Addenda). Participants gave written informed consent before taking part in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1. 

More details of the model and input variables. Table S1. Spearman’s Rank Correlation between the online hearing test and pure-tone audiometry over four frequencies and split by ear (n=93). Table S2. Summary of variables in the NHANES dataset, split into a tinnitus group and a non-tinnitus group. Fig. S1. Correlation matrix of the relevant variables in the NHANES dataset. Table S3. Summary of the simple and complex model of the NHANES data.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Reisinger, L., Schmidt, F., Benz, K. et al. Ageing as risk factor for tinnitus and its complex interplay with hearing loss—evidence from online and NHANES data. BMC Med 21, 283 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-023-02998-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-023-02998-1

Keywords