Skip to main content

Are Dutch adults equally susceptible to nudging and pricing strategies? Secondary analyses of the Supreme Nudge parallel cluster-randomised controlled supermarket trial

Abstract

Background

Supermarket interventions are promising to promote healthier dietary patterns, but not all individuals may be equally susceptible. We explored whether the effectiveness of nudging and pricing strategies on diet quality differs by psychological and grocery shopping characteristics.

Methods

We used data of the 12-month Supreme Nudge parallel cluster-randomised controlled supermarket trial, testing nudging and pricing strategies to promote healthier diets. Participants were Dutch speaking adults aged 30–80 years and regular shoppers of participating supermarkets (n = 12) in socially disadvantaged neighbourhoods. Data on psychological characteristics (food-related behaviours; price sensitivity; food decision styles; social cognitive factors; self-control) and grocery shopping characteristics (time spent in the supermarket; moment of the day; average supermarket visits; shopping at other retailers; supermarket proximity) were self-reported at baseline. These characteristics were tested for their moderating effects of the intervention on diet quality (scored 0–150) in linear mixed models.

Results

We included 162 participants from intervention supermarkets and 199 from control supermarkets (73% female, 58 (± 10.8) years old, 42% highly educated). The interventions had no overall effect on diet quality. Only five out of 23 potential moderators were statistically significant. Yet, stratified analyses of these significant moderators showed no significant effects on diet quality for one of the subgroups and statistically non-significant negative effects for the other. Negative effects were suggested for individuals with lower baseline levels of meal planning (β − 2.6, 95% CI − 5.9; 0.8), healthy shopping convenience (β − 3.0, 95% CI − 7.2; 1.3), and healthy food attractiveness (β − 3.5, 95% CI − 8.3; 1.3), and with higher levels of price consciousness (β − 2.6, 95% CI − 6.2; 1.0) and weekly supermarket visits (β − 2.4, 95% CI − 6.8; 1.9).

Conclusions

Adults with varying psychological and grocery shopping characteristics largely seem equally (un)susceptible to nudging and pricing strategies. It might be that certain characteristics lead to adverse effects, but this is not plausible, and the observed negative effects were small and statistically non-significant and may be explained by chance findings. Verification of these findings is needed in real-world trials based on larger sample sizes and with the use of more comprehensive interventions.

Trial registration

Dutch Trial Register ID NL7064, 30th of May, 2018, https://onderzoekmetmensen.nl/en/trial/20990

Peer Review reports

Background

Chronic diseases, such as cardiovascular diseases and type 2 diabetes, are highly prevalent and the leading causes of death globally [1, 2]. It is well established that adhering to a healthy dietary pattern can reduce chronic disease risk [3,4,5,6,7], and supermarket nudging and pricing strategies have frequently been proposed as solutions to promote healthier population diets. Nudging refers to environmental changes that promote a certain choice without removing the alternative choice [8]. Examples of supermarket nudges are replacements of confectionery products at the check-out counter by healthy snacks, or placing healthier products on eye-level. Evidence from systematic reviews in real-world purchasing settings demonstrates that nudges have, on average, modest but positive effects on promoting healthy product purchases [9,10,11]. Pricing strategies to promote healthy products are likely most effective when implemented as price decreases on healthy products and price increases on unhealthy products [12, 13]. The most well-known pricing strategy is a sugar tax to discourage sugar-sweetened beverage purchases, which has proven to be effective [14, 15].

Nudging and pricing strategies target different decision-making processes of supermarket customers. Based on the dual-process theory, decisions can be divided into automatic and fast decisions (referred to as System 1), and controlled and slow decisions (System 2) [16]. Nudges target System 1, for example, by making certain products more noticeable to trigger subconscious grocery decisions. Pricing strategies likely tap into System 2, as they require individuals to deliberately (re)consider the price in their purchasing decision. When pricing strategies are combined with promotional signs it can be seen as a nudge on top of the pricing strategy, thus relying on both System 1 and System 2. In that case, System 1 (promotional sign attracts attention) may trigger activation of System 2 ((re)considering the price), and their combination may yield the largest effects on purchasing decisions.

It is likely that not all individuals are equally susceptible to nudging and pricing strategies, as certain psychological characteristics may interact with potential nudging and pricing effects. For example, higher levels of price sensitivity might lead to increased activation of System 2 and subsequent higher susceptibility to pricing strategies, while lower levels of price sensitivity might result in higher reliance on System 1 and thus higher nudge susceptibility. Theory indicates that those with a low motivation for a certain message, low habit strength, or higher levels of impulsivity may rely more on System 1 leading to higher nudge susceptibility [17, 18]. On the other hand, high levels of health consciousness, habit strength, and strong product preferences may inhibit the activation of System 1, and thus attenuate nudge susceptibility [19]. Other characteristics such as higher levels of cooking skills or meal planning might also attenuate nudge susceptibility, since having the higher levels of these characteristics likely enhances relying on System 2.

The limited research on this topic thus far shows higher nudge susceptibility in those with low habit strength [20], low self-control [21], and non-dieters [22], and among those giving low priority to weight control [23]. Moreover, research suggests that price changes are equally effective across different levels of impulsivity, financial constraint, perceived stress, price sensitivity, intuitive and spontaneous decision-making styles, but more effective for those who give low priority to the food choice motives “natural content of foods” and “weight control” [23,24,25]. In addition, only one study thus far explored the moderating effects of various psychological characteristics for the combination of nudging and pricing strategies. It suggested equal positive effects on healthier purchasing outcomes across individuals with differing levels of impulsivity, price sensitivity, intuitive and spontaneous decision-making styles, and food choice motives [23]. However, this study was conducted in an experimental virtual supermarket environment and it remains to be determined whether these observations can be translated to real-world purchasing settings.

Whether variation in grocery shopping habits of customers may moderate nudging and pricing effectiveness is currently unclear. For example, some grocery shopping characteristics such as spending more time in the supermarket, shopping multiple times per week, and less frequent shopping at other food retailers may lead to higher exposure to interventional strategies, which potentially increases their effectiveness.

Equity-promoting effects, potentially with the largest effects in the most vulnerable groups, of nudging and pricing strategies across individuals with different characteristics would be desirable. It would indicate that the investigated subgroups could all benefit while reducing inequity, or are all equally unaffected, and provide insight into whether nudging and pricing strategies are suitable as general health-promoting strategies. Also, it is important to know if nudging and pricing strategies potentially lead to adverse effects (i.e. lower diet quality) among certain subgroups, which could hinder recommendations to implement these strategies on a large scale.

In our earlier work, we have shown that co-created supermarket nudging and pricing strategies were unable to change diet quality and that intervention effects were modified by age but not educational attainment and sex [26]. We now explore whether and how the effectiveness of nudging and pricing strategies on diet quality may differ by various psychological and grocery shopping characteristics, among Dutch adults in a real-world supermarket setting.

Methods

Trial design

For these secondary analyses, we used data from the Supreme Nudge parallel cluster-randomised controlled supermarket trial [27, 28]. The Supreme Nudge trial was implemented in 12 supermarkets located in socially disadvantaged neighbourhoods across the Netherlands. Stores were randomised to either the control group (n = 6) or the intervention group receiving nudging and pricing strategies to promote healthy products (n = 6). The trial ran from 6 months (n = 4) to 1 year (n = 8), depending on supermarket enrolment date.

Participants

Participant inclusion criteria were as follows: aged 30–80 years at the time of registration, living in the socially disadvantaged neighbourhood of a participating supermarket, being a regular shopper at a participating supermarket (> 50% weekly groceries at a selected store), planning on continuing shopping there for the study period, and having the ability to communicate in Dutch.

Recruitment

A stepwise participant recruitment strategy was used [29]. Recruitment started with passive strategies, including online news articles in local newspapers, supermarket flyers, e-mail invites to supermarket customer panels, in-store posters and other locations surrounding the supermarket (e.g. physiotherapist practices), postal invitation letters to every household in the supermarkets’ neighbourhoods, advertisement on the website of the study funder (Dutch Heart Foundation), and a social media campaign. Next, active recruitment strategies included asking participants to invite their partner or neighbours for participation, and in-store recruitment by the research team.

Interventions

Detailed information on the applied interventions is previously published [26]. Briefly, the supermarket interventions consisted of nudging and pricing strategies to promote healthier purchasing, which were developed according to a co-creative process with supermarket stakeholders and interventionists [27, 30]. Nudging and pricing strategies promoted healthy food groups which are recommended in the Dutch dietary guidelines [31]. Applied nudges on healthy products targeted 9% of the supermarket assortment and can be divided into placement nudges, focusing on availability and position, and into property nudges, focusing on presentation and information [32]. Placement nudges consisted of increased shelf space of healthier products and changing their location in the store. The property nudges focused on information symbols which highlighted the product’s tastiness, convenience or popularity, which were also used on different forms of promotional materials. Pricing strategies targeted 3% of the supermarket assortment and consisted of price decreases of healthy products and price increases of unhealthy products. Wherever possible, pricing strategies were implemented within the same food group. Price decreases were − 25%, or − 10% when combined with price increases (+ 15%) in the same food group. For example, fresh fruit and vegetables were − 25%, and whole-grain pasta products were − 10% with a simultaneous + 15% on the non-whole grain pasta (Additional file 1: Table S1).

Randomisation and blinding

The 12 supermarkets were cluster-randomised by the research team to the control group or the intervention group via a web-based random number generator tool in blocks of four. Blinding of participants was not possible due to the nature of the nudging and pricing strategies, Yet, participants were not, prior to participation nor during the study, actively informed of their supermarket allocation.

Data collection

Measurements took place at baseline (T0), after 3 months (T1), 6 months (T2), and 12 months (T3). Questionnaires were used to collect data on diet quality (T0, T1, T2, and T3) [33, 34], population characteristics (T0), and psychological characteristics and grocery shopping characteristics (T0) as potential moderators: food-related behaviours [35], price sensitivity [36], food decision styles [37,38,39,40,41], social cognitive factors related to healthy eating [42,43,44,45,46,47], self-control [48], time spent in the supermarket, moment of the day grocery shopping, average weekly supermarket visits, shopping at other food retailers, and supermarket proximity. Details on data collection methods and validity of used measurements have been previously described [27, 28], and a summary is provided in Table 1.

Table 1 Collected data and their operationalisation

Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics for the sociodemographic variables, diet quality score at T0 and the potential moderators were reported separately by the trial arm. Continuous variables with normal distributions were reported by their mean and standard deviation (SD), or by the median and interquartile range (IQR) in case of non-normality. Dichotomous and categorical variables were described by frequencies and percentages.

We used linear mixed models with group allocation as the independent variable and the diet quality score at T1, T2 and T3 as the dependent variable, including diet quality at T0 and time (categorical) as covariates. All models included random intercepts on the participant and on the supermarket level, based on intra-cluster correlation coefficients (ICCs) > 0 in the crude model without an interaction term. Interactions were tested between the group allocation and all potential moderators (food-related behaviours, price sensitivity, food decision styles, social cognitive factors related to healthy eating, self-control, and grocery shopping characteristics).

We used all available data of those participants who completed the baseline questionnaire, and the baseline measurement of diet quality with at least one follow-up measurement. Participants with missing data on certain moderators were excluded from the analysis involving those specific moderators. The absence of 0 in the 90% confidence interval (CI) was deemed a significant interaction considering the original sample size of the Supreme Nudge trial was not powered for subgroup analyses [27]. We did not account for multiple testing since our analyses were pre-planned in our protocol paper [27] and are of an explorative nature [49].

We report the regression coefficients (β) and 90% CI’s of the interaction terms as study outcomes, and stratified results are presented for significant moderators. Stratified subgroups were created according to the median, in which the median value was used in the upper category of each subgroup comparison. Effects within subgroups are reported by regression coefficients and 95% CI’s, and not 90% CI’s, to provide insight into the robustness of the findings.

Analyses were performed in R statistical software (version 4.3.2) using the lme4 package.

Results

In total, 361 participants completed the baseline questionnaire and at least one of the follow-up measurements of diet quality (Additional file 1: Figure S1). The sociodemographic characteristics of participants were equally distributed between the control group (n = 199) and the intervention group (n = 162). The study sample consisted of 73% females, with a mean age of 58 (± 10.8) years, and 42% were highly educated. Mean scores on diet quality and all potential moderators were comparable between the groups (Table 2). In both groups, an approximately even proportion of participants shared a household, with 12% (n = 24) in the control group and 9% (n = 14) in the intervention group. The ICCs were 0.39 and 0.04 for the clustering of data within participants and of participants within supermarkets, respectively.

Table 2 Study population baseline characteristics of the Supreme Nudge trial (n = 361)

As previously reported, the nudging and pricing strategies had no overall effect on diet quality (β − 1.1 (95% CI 3.8 to 1.7)) [26]. Most of the explored moderators did not show significant interaction effects with the intervention group, but five out of 23 did (Table 3). Compared to control participants, participants in the intervention group had for each unit increase in the baseline levels of meal planning a 6-point (90% CI 0.2 to 11.5) higher diet quality after 12 months of follow-up. Moreover, a higher diet quality was observed for participants with each unit increase in experienced healthy shopping convenience (β 1.9 (0.2 to 3.6)), and healthy food attractiveness (β 1.9 (0.4 to 3.3)), and for each unit decrease in price consciousness (β − 1.2 (− 2.2 to − 0.1)), and average number of weekly supermarket visits (β − 1.6 (− 2.7 to − 0.5)).

Table 3 Interaction between nudging and pricing strategies and potential moderators on diet quality (n = 361)

Stratified analyses of significant moderators showed, for all moderators, no effects for one of the subgroups and a modest and statistically non-significant negative effect on diet quality for the other (Table 4). Negative effects were suggested for individuals with lower baseline levels of meal planning, experienced healthy shopping convenience, and healthy food attractiveness, and with higher levels of price consciousness and weekly supermarket visits. For example, for participants characterised as having lower levels of meal planning, intervention exposure led to a − 2.6 points (95% CI − 5.9 to 0.8) lower diet quality compared to the control group, while for those characterised as having higher levels of meal planning intervention exposure did not change diet quality (β − 0.1 (− 2.8 to 2.9)) compared to the control group.

Table 4 Stratified analyses of significant moderators of nudging and pricing strategies on diet quality (n = 361)

Discussion

This study showed that the effect of nudging and pricing strategies in real-world supermarkets on diet quality was not moderated by most of the explored psychological and grocery shopping characteristics. In addition, findings suggest a modest negative effect on diet quality after exposure to nudging and pricing strategies for individuals with lower baseline levels of meal planning, healthy shopping convenience, and experienced healthy food attractiveness, and with higher baseline levels of price consciousness and number of weekly supermarket visits.

Most studies on the moderating effects of psychological characteristics investigated the isolated effect of nudging or pricing strategies [18, 20,21,22, 24, 50]. We used a combined intervention of nudging and pricing strategies for which the theoretical foundation differs: nudging relies on unconscious decisions while pricing strategies rely more on conscious decisions [16]. Nonetheless, our results build upon the earlier findings from a simulated virtual supermarket experiment in which the investigated psychological characteristics impulsivity, price sensitivity, decision-making styles, and food choice motives did not moderate the effect of nudging and pricing strategies [23]. Our findings are also in line with the previous observation that healthy eating motivation does not seem to be a moderator [50]. However, in contrast to previous observations from simulated study settings [17, 18, 20, 21, 24], we did not observe that lower levels of self-control, conscious grocery shopping, and habit strength led to higher intervention susceptibility in a real-world setting. The influence of variation in psychological characteristics across individuals and their response to the nudging and pricing strategies may be overruled by the many other (marketing) stimuli present in a real-world supermarket setting which are driving purchasing behaviours [51]. It may also be overruled by strong product preferences or habitual shopping patterns in the supermarket which individuals may use to minimise the time and mental capacity required for grocery shopping. In the present study, we had limited insight in habitual shopping patterns. Indeed, we measured habitual shopping related to vegetables and snack purchases, but, for example, not to other food groups or specific brands, or overall grocery shopping habits (e.g. use of grocery lists or habitual store routing).

The potentially negative effects of nudging and pricing strategies on diet quality among the five statistically significant moderators are not easily explained. It might be that certain characteristics lead to adverse effects, but this is not plausible and it should be noted that the observed negative effects were small and statistically non-significant, and may be explained by chance findings. Verification of these findings is thus crucial. Nevertheless, it might be that those with lower levels of experienced convenience of healthy shopping and experienced healthy food attractiveness somewhat oppose of healthier products or do not want to be patronised by the nudging and pricing strategies. Lower diet quality for those with higher levels of price consciousness may be explained by having a lower income to spend on groceries compared to those with lower price consciousness. Lower diet quality among those with more frequent supermarket visits per week in general — not specifically to a participating study supermarket — might be due to lower intervention exposure in number of minutes per visit and throughout the different supermarket sections, as more frequent visits may mean more visits to different supermarket chains.

The observations that most of the investigated moderators did not influence susceptibility to nudging and pricing strategies and that significant moderators showed relatively small effect sizes are promising for these interventions as a general public health strategy. It will likely not increase dietary inequalities between subgroups with varying psychological and grocery shopping characteristics. Verification of our findings is needed in future real-world randomised controlled supermarket trials based on larger sample sizes and with the use of more comprehensive intervention strategies. Strategies should, besides promoting healthy purchases, discourage unhealthy purchases (e.g. no marketing and promotions of unhealthy products, elimination of unhealthy products at checkouts, and limited availability of unhealthy products) [52, 53]. Moreover, future real-world trials should further investigate the equity of nudging and pricing strategies by investigating sociodemographic characteristics such as income and ethnicity as moderators. Especially income is a known moderator for pricing strategies, but whether low or high-income groups are more responsive to pricing strategies is not consistently shown [24]. Lastly, to create impactful and sustainable changes in supermarkets, there is an urgent need for policy measures that create a level playing field among food retailers to overcome commercial barriers that currently hinder impactful changes [54].

Strengths and limitations

This study was based on a strong cluster-randomised and controlled trial design, securing a high internal validity. It was the first of its kind by investigating the combination of nudging and pricing strategies and potential moderating factors in a real-world purchasing setting. In addition, the longitudinal design enabled us to determine the moderation effects based on within-subject and between-subject average changes in diet quality. However, some limitations should be acknowledged. First, our study power was likely relatively low as the original sample size of the Supreme Nudge trial was not powered for subgroup analyses, although exploring for effect modification was planned in our study protocol [27]. We aimed to address the lack of power via the use of a 90% CI to determine significant interactions, which could have led to a less precise estimation of interaction effects. Second, the external validity may be limited due to a 10 points higher diet quality score at baseline than is observed in a cross-sectional Dutch population sample [55]. This may have attenuated the intervention effects and thus the ability to detect potential moderators. Third, measurement of dietary intake via quantitative questionnaires is prone to over- or underreporting and socially desirable answers. The diet quality score based on the short 40-item food frequency questionnaire (FFQ) was validated against a 180-item FFQ combined with a 24-h urinary sodium excretion value, which revealed a moderate correlation of 0.56 [33]. Yet, our analyses are likely minimally affected by this measurement error since we adjusted for the diet quality baseline value. Fourth, this intervention combined nudging and pricing strategies but the implementation of nudging versus pricing strategies was outbalanced. As this was a real-world trial, in which the intervention component development was based on a co-creative process with the participating supermarket chain, the number of nudging types and their implementation across various food groups were outweighing the smaller absolute number of implemented pricing strategies — which were based on a maximum of 200 price changes per week. An unequally balanced intervention dosage may have influenced our findings, since nudging and pricing strategies rely on different theoretical foundations and food prices are known to be a strong driver of food purchasing decisions [12].

Conclusions

Dutch adults with varying psychological and grocery shopping characteristics seem to a large extent equally (un)susceptible to nudging and pricing strategies. Stratified analyses suggested a modest and statistically non-significant negative effect on diet quality after exposure to nudging and pricing strategies for individuals with lower baseline levels of meal planning, healthy shopping convenience, and experienced healthy food attractiveness, and with higher baseline levels of price consciousness and number of weekly supermarket visits. These potential adverse effects are not plausible and the observed negative effects were small and statistically non-significant and may be explained by chance findings. Verification of these findings is needed in future real-world randomised controlled supermarket trials based on larger sample sizes and with the use of more comprehensive intervention strategies.

Availability of data and materials

The data analysed during the current study are not publicly available as it will violate participant consent. The analysis plan and analytical code are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

CI:

Confidence interval

FFQ:

Food frequency questionnaire

References

  1. Roth GA, Mensah GA, Johnson CO, Addolorato G, Ammirati E, Baddour LM, et al. Global burden of cardiovascular diseases and risk factors, 1990–2019 update from the GBD 2019 study. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2020;76(25):2982–3021.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Safiri S, Karamzad N, Kaufman JS, Bell AW, Nejadghaderi SA, Sullman MJM, et al. Prevalence, deaths and Disability-Adjusted-Life-Years (DALYs) due to type 2 diabetes and its attributable risk factors in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: results from the global burden of disease study 2019. Front Endocrinol. 2022;13:1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Micha R, Shulkin ML, Penalvo JL, Khatibzadeh S, Singh GM, Rao M, et al. Etiologic effects and optimal intakes of foods and nutrients for risk of cardiovascular diseases and diabetes: systematic reviews and meta-analyses from the Nutrition and Chronic Diseases Expert Group (NutriCoDE). Plos One. 2017;12(4):1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Rodriguez-Monforte M, Flores-Mateo G, Sanchez E. Dietary patterns and CVD: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Brit J Nutr. 2015;114(9):1341–59.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Chiavaroli L, Viguiliouk E, Nishi SK, Mejia SB, Rahelic D, Kahleova H, et al. DASH dietary pattern and cardiometabolic outcomes: an umbrella review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Nutrients. 2019;11(2):1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Martinez-Lacoba R, Pardo-Garcia I, Amo-Saus E, Escribano-Sotos F. Mediterranean diet and health outcomes: a systematic meta-review. Eur J Public Health. 2018;28(5):955–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Threapleton DE, Greenwood DC, Evans CEL, Cleghorn CL, Nykjaer C, Woodhead C, et al. Dietary fibre intake and risk of cardiovascular disease: systematic review and meta-analysis. Bmj-Brit Med J. 2013;347(f6879):1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Thaler RH, Sunstein CR. Nudge: improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. New Haven: Yale University Press; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Harbers MC, Beulens JWJ, Rutters F, de Boer F, Gillebaart M, Sluijs I, van der Schouw YT. The effects of nudges on purchases, food choice, and energy intake or content of purchases in real-life food purchasing environments: a systematic review and evidence synthesis. Nutr J. 2020;19(1):103.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Hartmann-Boyce J, Bianchi F, Piernas C, Riches SP, Frie K, Nourse R, Jebb SA. Grocery store interventions to change food purchasing behaviors: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Am J Clin Nutr. 2018;107(6):1004–16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Atanasova P, Kusuma D, Pineda E, Frost G, Sassi F, Miraldo M. The impact of the consumer and neighbourhood food environment on dietary intake and obesity-related outcomes: a systematic review of causal impact studies. Soc Sci Med. 2022;299:114879.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Afshin A, Penalvo J, Del Gobbo L, Kashaf M, Micha R, Morrish K, et al. CVD Prevention through policy: a review of mass media, food/menu labeling, taxation/subsidies, built environment, school procurement, worksite wellness, and marketing standards to improve diet. Curr Cardiol Rep. 2015;17(11):98.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Mozaffarian D, Afshin A, Benowitz NL, Bittner V, Daniels SR, Franch HA, et al. Population approaches to improve diet, physical activity, and smoking habits a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2012;126(12):1514.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Royo-Bordonada MA, Fernandez-Escobar C, Gil-Bellosta CJ, Ordaz E. Effect of excise tax on sugar-sweetened beverages in Catalonia, Spain, three and a half years after its introduction. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2022;19(1):24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Colchero MA, Popkin BM, Rivera JA, Ng SW. Beverage purchases from stores in Mexico under the excise tax on sugar sweetened beverages: observational study. BMJ. 2016;352:h6704.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Evans JSBT. Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:255–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Cacioppo JT, Kao CF, Petty RE, Rodriguez R. Central and peripheral routes to persuasion - an individual difference perspective. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51(5):1032–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. de Ridder D, Kroese F, van Gestel L. Nudgeability: mapping conditions of susceptibility to nudge influence. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2022;17(2):346–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Bucher T, Collins C, Rollo ME, McCaffrey TA, De Vlieger N, Van der Bend D, et al. Nudging consumers towards healthier choices: a systematic review of positional influences on food choice. Brit J Nutr. 2016;115(12):2252–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Venema TAG, Kroese FM, Verplanken B, de Ridder DTD. The (bitter) sweet taste of nudge effectiveness: the role of habits in a portion size nudge, a proof of concept study. Appetite. 2020;151(1):104699.

  21. Koenigstorfer J, Groeppel-Klein A, Kamm F. Healthful food decision making in response to traffic light color-coded nutrition labeling. J Public Policy Mark. 2014;33(1):65–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Trudel R, Murray KB, Kim S, Chen S. The impact of traffic light color-coding on food health perceptions and choice. J Exp Psychol-Appl. 2015;21(3):255–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. van der Molen AEH, Hoenink JC, Mackenbach JD, Waterlander W, Lakerveld J, Beulens JWJ. Are nudging and pricing strategies on food purchasing behaviors equally effective for all? Secondary analyses from the Supreme Nudge virtual supermarket study. Appetite. 2021;167(1):105655.

  24. Mizdrak A, Scarborough P, Waterlander WE, Rayner M. Differential responses to food price changes by personal characteristic: a systematic review of experimental studies. Plos One. 2015;10(7):1–19.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Djojosoeparto SK, Poelman MP, Eykelenboom M, Beenackers MA, Steenhuis IH, van Stralen MM, et al. Do financial constraint and perceived stress modify the effects of food tax schemes on food purchases: moderation analyses in a virtual supermarket experiment. Public Health Nutr. 2024;27(1):e38.

  26. Stuber JM, Mackenbach JD, de Bruijn GJ, Gillebaart M, Hoenink JC, Middel CNH, et al. Real-world nudging, pricing, and mobile physical activity coaching was insufficient to improve lifestyle behaviours and cardiometabolic health: the Supreme Nudge parallel cluster-randomised controlled supermarket trial. BMC Med. 2024;22(1):52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Stuber JM, Mackenbach JD, de Boer FE, de Bruijn GJ, Gillebaart M, Harbers MC, et al. Reducing cardiometabolic risk in adults with a low socioeconomic position: protocol of the Supreme Nudge parallel cluster-randomised controlled supermarket trial. Nutr J. 2020;19(1):46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Stuber JM, Mackenbach JD, de Boer FE, de Bruijn GJ, Gillebaart M, Harbers MC, et al. Correction: Reducing cardiometabolic risk in adults with a low socioeconomic position: protocol of the Supreme Nudge parallel cluster-randomised controlled supermarket trial. Nutr J. 2022;21(1):44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Stuber JM, Hoek BACE, Vos AL, Smit EG, Lakerveld J, Mackenbach JD, Beulens JWJ. Participant recruitment, baseline characteristics and at-home-measurements of cardiometabolic risk markers: insights from the Supreme Nudge parallel cluster-randomised controlled supermarket trial. Trials. 2023;24(159):1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Middel CNH, Schuitmaker-Warnaar TJ, Mackenbach JD, Broerse JEW. Designing a healthy food-store intervention; a co-creative process between interventionists and supermarket actors. Int J Health Policy. 2021;11(10):2175–88.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Kromhout D, Spaaij CJK, de Goede J, Weggemans RM, Brug J, Geleijnse JM, et al. The 2015 Dutch food-based dietary guidelines. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2016;70(8):869–78.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Hollands GJ, Bignardi G, Johnston M, Kelly MP, Ogilvie D, Petticrew M, et al. The TIPPME intervention typology for changing environments to change behaviour. Nat Hum Behav. 2017;1(8):0140.

  33. van Lee L, Feskens EJ, Meijboom S, Hooft van Huysduynen EJ, van’t Veer P, de Vries JH, Geelen A. Evaluation of a screener to assess diet quality in the Netherlands. Br J Nutr. 2016;115(3):517–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Looman M, Feskens EJ, de Rijk M, Meijboom S, Biesbroek S, Temme EH, et al. Development and evaluation of the Dutch Healthy Diet index 2015. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20(13):2289–99.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Crawford D, Ball K, Mishra G, Salmon J, Timperio A. Which food-related behaviours are associated with healthier intakes of fruits and vegetables among women? (vol 10, pg 256, 2007). Public Health Nutr. 2007;10(5):536.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Lichtenstein DR, Ridgway NM, Netemeyer RG. Price perceptions and consumer shopping behavior - a field-study. J Marketing Res. 1993;30(2):234–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Verplanken B, Herabadi A. Individual differences in impulse buying tendency: Feeling and no thinking. Eur J Personality. 2001;15:S71–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Spinella M. Normative data and a short form of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Int J Neurosci. 2007;117(3):359–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Scott SG, Bruce RA. Decision-Making Style - the Development and Assessment of a New Measure. Educ Psychol Meas. 1995;55(5):818–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Verplanken B, Orbell S. Reflections on past behavior: A self-report index of habit strength. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2003;33(6):1313–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Ersche KD, Lim TV, Ward LHE, Robbins TW, Stochl J. Creature of Habit: A self-report measure of habitual routines and automatic tendencies in everyday life. Pers Indiv Differ. 2017;116:73–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. de Ridder D, Gillebaart M. What’s in a nudge. Tijdschrift voor gezondheidswetenschappen. 2016;94(7):261–5.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Forwood SE, Ahern AL, Hollands GJ, Ng YL, Marteau TM. Priming healthy eating. You can’t prime all the people all of the time. Appetite. 2015;89:93–102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. GiskeS K, Van Lenthe FJ, Brug J, Mackenbach JP, Turrell G. Socioeconomic inequalities in food purchasing: The contribution of respondent-perceived and actual (objectively measured) price and availability of foods. Prev Med. 2007;45(1):41–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. van Ansem WJC, Schrijvers CTM, Rodenburg G, van de Mheen D. Is there an association between the home food environment, the local food shopping environment and children’s fruit and vegetable intake? Results from the Dutch INPACT study. Public Health Nutr. 2013;16(7):1206–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Higgs S, Liu J, Collins EIM, Thomas JM. Using social norms to encourage healthier eating. Nutr Bull. 2019;44(1):43–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Dijkstra SC, Neter JE, van Stralen MM, Knol DL, Brouwer IA, Huisman M, Visser M. The role of perceived barriers in explaining socio-economic status differences in adherence to the fruit, vegetable and fish guidelines in older adults: a mediation study. Public Health Nutr. 2015;18(5):797–808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Mattes R, Foster GD. Food environment and obesity. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2014;22(12):2459–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Bender R, Lange S. Adjusting for multiple testing–when and how? J Clin Epidemiol. 2001;54(4):343–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Aschemann-Witzel J, Grunert KG, van Trijp HCM, Bialkova S, Raats MM, Hodgkins C, et al. Effects of nutrition label format and product assortment on the healthfulness of food choice. Appetite. 2013;71:63–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Marteau TM, Hollands GJ, Fletcher PC. Changing Human Behavior to Prevent Disease: The Importance of Targeting Automatic Processes. Science. 2012;337(6101):1492–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. WHO. European Regional Obesity Report 2022. Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe; 2022.

  53. Jacobson MF, Krieger J, Brownell KD. Potential policy approaches to address diet-related diseases. JAMA. 2018;320(4):341–2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Middel CNH, Schuitmaker-Warnaar TJ, Mackenbach JD, Broerse JEW. Systematic review: a systems innovation perspective on barriers and facilitators for the implementation of healthy food-store interventions. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2019;16(1):108.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Hoenink JC, Waterlander W, Beulens JWJ, Mackenbach JD. The role of material and psychosocial resources in explaining socioeconomic inequalities in diet: A structural equation modelling approach. SSM Popul Health. 2022;17:101025.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the supermarket chain and study participants for their participation in the Supreme Nudge trial.

In addition to the authors, SUPREME NUDGE consortium partners who contributed to the trial conception were: Jody C. Hoenink, University of Cambridge; Femke Rutters, Amsterdam UMC, VU University; Wilma E. Waterlander, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam; Denise T. D. de Ridder, Utrecht University; Marleen Gillebaart, Utrecht University; Stephanie Blom, Utrecht University; Femke E. de Boer, Utrecht University; Gert-Jan de Bruijn, University of Antwerp; Anne L. Vos, University of Amsterdam; Edith G. Smit, University of Amsterdam; Michel C. A. Klein, VU University; Jacqueline E. W. Broerse, VU University; Tjerk-Jan Schuitmaker-Warnaar, VU University; Cédric N. H. Middel, VU University; Yvonne T. van der Schouw, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht University; Ivonne Sluijs, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht University; Marjolein C. Harbers, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht University; Elizabeth Velema, Netherlands Nutrition Centre.

Funding

This study was part of the ‘Sustainable Prevention of Cardiometabolic Risk through Nudging Health Behaviors’ (Supreme Nudge) project. The Supreme Nudge project is funded by the Dutch Heart Foundation (grant number CVON2016-04) and the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMw) (531003001). JMS and JDM are currently funded by the Dutch Heart Foundation and ZonMw in collaboration with and supported by the Dutch CardioVascular Allicance (01–001-2021-B017). Funders had no role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript, nor have they authority on the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Consortia

Contributions

JWJB, JL, and JDM were initiators of the Supreme Nudge trial and, together with JMS, were major contributors in its conceptual design. JMS initiated the current study, coordinated data collection, conducted the analysis, and drafted the manuscript. JJEL and ES assisted with data analysis and manuscript drafting. JDM supervised the current project. All authors critically read and approved of the final manuscript and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Josine M. Stuber.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study protocol of the Supreme Nudge trial was approved by the Medical Ethics Review Committee of VU University Medical Center in Amsterdam, the Netherlands (reference number: 2019.334) and written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to study enrolment.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

12916_2024_3429_MOESM1_ESM.docx

Additional file 1: Table S1. Overview of implemented supermarket interventions in the Supreme Nudge trial. Figure S1. Participant flowchart.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stuber, J.M., Beulens, J.W.J., van Lierop, J.J.E. et al. Are Dutch adults equally susceptible to nudging and pricing strategies? Secondary analyses of the Supreme Nudge parallel cluster-randomised controlled supermarket trial. BMC Med 22, 228 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-024-03429-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-024-03429-5

Keywords