Skip to main content

Virtual reality as an engaging and enjoyable method for delivering emergency clinical simulation training: a prospective, interventional study of medical undergraduates

Abstract

Background

It is a requirement that medical students are educated in emergencies and feel well prepared for practice as a doctor, yet national surveys show that many students feel underprepared. Virtual reality (VR), combined with 360-degree filming, provides an immersive, realistic, and interactive simulation experience. Unlike conventional in-person simulation, it is scalable with reduced workforce demands. We sought to compare students’ engagement and enjoyment of VR simulation to desktop computer-based simulation.

Methods

We conducted a prospective, interventional, evaluation study. The study was carried out on final year medical students undertaking their Pre-Foundation Assistantship (n = 116) at Imperial College School of Medicine (ICSM) in London. We compared objective engagement, subjective engagement, and subjective enjoyment of VR simulation to desktop computer-based simulation using cardiac arrest and life-threatening asthma scenarios. Engagement was measured objectively using students’ physiological parameters, including heart rate and eye tracking, and facilitator observations using the validated ‘Behavioural Engagement Related to Instruction’ (BERI) protocol. Students’ subjective engagement and enjoyment levels were measured using a post-session survey.

Results

Students’ maximum heart rates were significantly higher during VR simulation with a mean difference of 4.2 beats per minute (3.2 to 5.2, p < 0.001), and eye tracking showed they spent a significantly greater mean percentage of time of 6.4% (5.1 to 7.7, p < 0.001) focusing on the scenarios in VR compared to standard desktop. Qualitative data showed students enjoyed and felt engaged with the sessions, which provided a safe space for learning.

Conclusions

Our study shows that students found VR simulations enjoyable and were more engaged compared to standard desktop simulation. This suggests that 360-degree VR simulation experiences provide students with immersive, realistic training, which is scalable, giving them the unique opportunity to manage emergencies and work within emergency teams, which would not typically occur during traditional training.

Peer Review reports

Background

Attrition of doctors, rota gaps and industrial action within the National Health Service are becoming increasingly common. Training more doctors is a frequently cited solution, yet the current learning environment is not geared for such capacity changes, and many existing medical students already feel unprepared for the technical and non-technical aspects of practice. Simulation-based medical education (SMBE) describes artificially reproducing authentic medical scenarios to accomplish educational outcomes. SBME utilises experiential learning to guide learners and is a teaching style that is commonly used in the medical curricula to effectively complement patient safety [1]. Complex, high-stakes emergency scenarios are often practiced and assessed in SBME to build self-confidence in learners during these demanding settings without any real consequence to patient safety [2]. In medicine, SMBE is typically delivered through in-person simulation, an effective but resource intensive teaching method [3], which is used sparingly due to timetabling and workforce pressures. Comparable industries, such as aviation, use highly sophisticated simulators to train pilots in a realistic and immersive manner. However, this type of simulation is expensive and therefore inaccessible in the healthcare setting. Computer-based simulation on a desktop is a seemingly inexpensive and accessible resource given most students have access to a computer personally or through institutions. It however lacks elements of immersion and engagement. Over the past two decades, SBME has advanced with evolving technology and more recently seen an adoption of virtual reality (VR) as a technique of technology-enhanced learning [4]. VR simulation combined with 360-degree filming may provide a sustainable option for simulation training. Learners can safely be immersed in, interact with and influence a realistic clinical setting, whilst also receiving core teaching [5, 6]. VR provides an opportunity for blended learning with existing in-person clinical placements and addresses the gaps in clinical exposure in a cost-effective and scalable manner. VR resources can be readily shared across healthcare sectors and educational establishments.

VR describes a human–computer interface that provides users with an immersive computer simulated environment. Immersion requires a high level of attention and describes being engaged in the environment to the point where all the focus is on that particular task and there is a strong feeling of being there [7]. This element is key in medical training where realism, rather than gamification, is valued and total immersion tests students’ ability to remain level headed in dynamic scenarios. VR is not new technology, with the term first being described by Jaron Lenier in the mid-1980s [8]. Over time, and with advancements in technology, VR has grown to be used commercially and its technology is well established in gaming. Studies have found that immersion with VR through experiential learning improves learning [9, 10]. Using VR has been shown to be more useful for education, and learners have subjectively found its use exciting or interesting [10, 11]. Learning with simulations has shown to be an effective teaching method for knowledge retention and overall student performance [12,13,14]. Higher levels of student engagement result in higher levels of academic achievement [15, 16]. We combined the two key features of simulation, realism and immersion, through 360-degree filming of a ‘real life’ emergency delivered interactively in VR to train clinical year medical students in managing cardiac arrest and life-threatening asthma.

Student engagement with teaching can be measured objectively and subjectively. Subjective methods include self-reported levels of engagement using engagement assessment tools [17, 18]. Objective methods of student engagement include monitoring physiological variables such as heart rate (HR) or eye movements [19,20,21] and validated observer tools of engagement such as the Behavioural Engagement Related to Instruction (BERI) protocol [22]. Heart rate levels are an effective monitor of student engagement levels with an increase in heart rate seen during periods of involvement or interaction [19, 20]. Eye movement information can provide valuable insight into engagement and attention levels. Movements can be defined as saccades, rapid eye movements from point-to-point at high velocities (> 300°/s) or fixations, low-velocity eye movements (< 100°/s), which indicate lingering of the gaze [23]. Overall, longer fixations imply greater cognitive processing and regular saccades typically indicate distraction from a task [24, 25]. Fixations can also take place within areas-of-interest (AOI), which are pre-determined target regions that subjects should be placing attention to and longer periods of eye movements within the AOIs indicate higher engagement [23, 26]. In this study, we sought to understand whether students found VR simulations engaging and enjoyable and how this compared to desktop computer-based simulation.

Methods

The Imperial College London Education Ethics Review Process (study ref. EERP2122-038) approved the study on 24 January 2022. We conducted a prospective interventional evaluation study of final year medical students (n = 116) from 27 April 2022 to 22 June 2022. The study was carried out at Imperial College School of Medicine (ICSM) in London. All final year medical students undertaking their Pre-Foundation Assistantship (PFA) at ICSM were screened for eligibility. Participants gave informed consent before taking part. All those who attended the simulations agreed to partake in the research, and it was clearly highlighted to them beforehand that those who chose not to participate in the research would still be given the same learning opportunities. Our inclusion criteria included students who were able to give informed consent. The exclusion criteria for eye tracking data included students who had undergone any eye surgery or students wearing bifocal/trifocal glasses for the study. Exclusion criteria for the heart rate data included participants with any pre-existing cardiac rhythm abnormalities. These conditions were excluded as they interfere with the eye tracking and the heart rate monitoring leading to inaccurate data. Students were provided with written information sheets and a verbal introduction with question-and-answer opportunities. The purpose of the study proposed to students was to understand whether VR simulation is an engaging and enjoyable form of simulation that allows them to experience emergency scenarios that they might not see in real life. Students were able to withdraw their information within 2 weeks.

The study aimed to compare student engagement and enjoyment levels between VR simulation and desktop computer-based simulation during two simulation experiences of a cardiac arrest and life-threatening asthma attack (see Additional File 1: Images S1 and S2). Both scenarios were 15 min and created using 360-degree filming of acted-out scenarios. Interactivity was introduced with questions and variable scene branching dictated by student responses. The format of the 20-min debriefs for both groups were the same, consisting of discussions around the technical and clinical skills involved in managing both scenarios and the non-technical skills involved. All students experienced both scenarios, and all students experienced VR once and standard desktop once. Students were split into groups of 10–15 for each of the VR group and the desktop group. Students did not know which scenario they would be presented with first. In the first phase of the study, students were randomly assigned to either experience cardiac arrest in VR or on standard desktop. In the second phase of the study, those who were previously in the VR group for cardiac arrest transitioned to life-threatening asthma on standard desktop, and those previously in the cardiac arrest standard desktop group transitioned to life-threatening asthma in VR. One hundred ninety-one datasets were collected in total (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Distribution of students into scenario type and delivery method sub-groups

Engagement was measured objectively using students’ physiological parameters (heart rate and eye tracking) and facilitator observations using the validated BERI protocol [22]. Heart rates were monitored using the Polar H10 heart rate sensor. Eye tracking data was obtained from Tobii VR4 platform with spotlight technology built into the Pico Neo 3 Pro Eye VR headsets. Eye tracking parameters measured included pupillary saccades and fixations within assigned areas of interest. Students’ subjective engagement and enjoyment levels were measured using a post-session survey which was collected immediately after the session using an online questionnaire and included a combination of qualitative and quantitative data.

Each of the simulations consisted of a baseline period, followed by alternating scenes and questions. Data on the students’ heart rate and whether they were on task (measured using eye tracking) were recorded serially over the simulations. For each student, the data was reduced to a single measurement per section of the simulation (either the baseline period, or individual questions and scenes). For heart rate monitoring, both the mean and maximum value during the section was calculated. For engagement, the percentage of measurements where the student was on-task was measured using eye tracking.

The analyses of both the VR data and the desktop data used equivalent methods. Due to the multiple measurements for each student over time, the analyses were performed using mixed (multilevel) linear regression models. The outcome variable was the heart rate/on task measurement for each section of the simulation (either scene or question). The equivalent outcome at baseline was used as covariate in the analyses. Also included as a covariate was the section number, to allow for trends in outcome over the course of the simulation. Two level regression models were used with individual measurements for each section nested within students. An autoregressive structure for the residuals was used, which allows each measurement to be correlated with the preceding measurement.

The first analysis of the joint VR and standard desktop data compared the difference between techniques for the ‘baseline’ measurements made before the main part of the simulation. One measurement per student was included in these analyses, with the unpaired t-test used for the comparisons. The regression analyses were adjusted for differences during the baseline period as there were differences during the baseline period.

The analysis of the main simulation outcomes was performed using similar statistical methods to those previously described. Initially, the difference between scenes and questions was examined for all scenarios combined. Subsequently, the interaction between technique (standard desktop or VR) and scenario (scenes or questions) was included. If the interaction was significant, this would suggest that the difference techniques varied for scenes and questions. Where a significant interaction was present, the technique differences were quantified for each scenario. If the interaction was not significant, it was omitted from the analysis, and a single difference between techniques was quantified.

Results

Engagement levels using physiological parameters

The heart rate values during the baseline period were significantly higher for the VR technique. This was adjusted for when analysing the main part of the simulation using regression analyses (see Additional File 2: Table S1). The results presented in Table 1 summarise the differences in outcome between standard desktop and VR methods. Summaries for each method are reported, along with a mean difference and corresponding confidence interval. As there were differences during the baseline period (see Additional File 2: Table S1), the adjusted differences presented in Table 1 are notably lower than the raw difference in outcome between groups. However, the fact that the differences were statistically significant implies that there were differences in outcome between techniques even after allowing for the baseline differences.

Table 1 Comparison of heart rate outcomes between standard desktop and VR groups after adjusting for differences during the baseline period using regression analyses

The mean of the mean heart rates was significantly higher during VR compared to desktop for both simulations combined, by 3.0 bpm (2.2 to 3.8, p < 0.001), after adjusting for baseline differences. The mean of the maximum heart rates was also significantly higher during VR compared to desktop for both simulations combined, by 4.2 bpm (3.2 to 5.2, p < 0.001), again, after adjusting for baseline differences. For time on task measured using eye tracking, the percentage of time on task was 6.4% higher (5.1 to 7.7, p < 0.001) for the VR technique than for standard desktop.

Observed engagement levels

When analysing the observed engagement levels of the 191 student experiences, students were engaged for a mean of 93.61% (SD = 0.048) of time when using VR, compared to 88.13% (SD = 0.087) of time for students using standard desktop. An independent samples T-test with a 95% confidence interval showed a statistically significant difference in observed engagement between the VR group and the desktop group (p = 0.001).

Subjective engagement and enjoyment levels

A total of 191 feedback datasets were collected on students’ subjective engagement and enjoyment levels using a five-point Likert-style questionnaire. A Pearson chi-square test was performed to determine whether the simulation scenario (cardiac arrest or life-threatening asthma) had any significant impact on engagement, and enjoyment levels gave a p-value of 0.265 and 0.494 respectively. We therefore concluded that there was no significant difference between the simulation scenarios with regard to the subjective engagement and enjoyment levels. The results of the simulations were then pooled to create two distinct groups: VR and standard desktop (Table 2). The average engagement level was 4.25 when using a standard desktop (SD = 0.88), compared to 4.48 when using VR (SD = 0.75). The average enjoyment level was 4.05 when using a standard desktop (SD = 0.96), compared to 4.44 using VR (SD = 0.76). A Pearson chi-square test was performed to determine whether the type of simulation (VR or standard desktop) had any significant impact on engagement, and enjoyment levels gave a p-value of 0.076 and 0.009, respectively.

Table 2 Comparison of subjective engagement and enjoyment levels between standard desktop and VR groups for both cardiac arrest and asthma scenarios combined

Discussion

It is undoubted that student engagement is important in education. An engaged student is likely to learn more and retain more [27]. Heart rate and engagement are closely correlated [28]. Superior learning performance [29], greater cognitive effort, and higher order problem solving are all correlated with an increased heart rate [30, 31]. Our results suggest a significant difference in mean and maximum heart rates during VR simulation compared to standard desktop simulation. Literature suggests that the heart rate change signifying engagement is typically around 5 bpm, which is mirrored in our study where the difference in the maximum heart rates between standard desktop and VR is 4.2 bpm [30]. Heart rates were monitored using a heart rate monitor, which was connected to a tablet for the desktop group, and to the VR headset via Bluetooth for the VR group. Interruptions in heart rate monitoring were therefore easier to detect and rectify for the desktop group where we could see the tablet, which explains why more students had heart rate monitoring data for the desktop group.

Blended learning is an established feature of undergraduate and postgraduate medical education. Studies confirm that students who watch videos and participate in classroom teaching perform better than those who only received traditional face-to-face teaching [32]. Furthermore, dynamic video-based learning improves performance compared to static text and pictures [33]. Eye tracking provides key physiological indicators of engagement and focus. Fixations define a location where a student is focusing their visual attention. When an individual is focusing on an area, the assumption is that they are retrieving and processing information in that area. We defined these as areas of interest, and the percentage of time students’ gaze was fixed on that area of interest was recorded. Our results show that students spend a significantly greater percentage of time focusing on the predefined areas of interest which reflect the scene or task in front of them (‘on task’) during VR simulation when compared to standard desktop.

Technical characteristics of the desktop computer and the VR glasses can affect the quality of the content, and we took steps to mitigate against this influencing user immersion. We used screens readily accessible to students, and whilst the VR headset had a higher resolution than the desktop, there was a loss in quality due to projection of the image onto a sphere in VR. Video framerates matched in both modalities and remained below the refresh rate. Video playback of the scenarios was used in VR instead of streaming, and scenarios were streamed using an Ethernet connection on the desktops to ensure there were no latency issues. The audio was delivered through identical headphones for both VR and desktop.

Our mixed methods study using physiological parameters, objective engagement observations, and subjective engagement and enjoyment levels shows that delivering simulation in the form of VR is more enjoyable and engaging than watching a 360-degree video on a desktop. This study is the first to evaluate the engagement and enjoyment of VR in the context of undergraduate medical training in an era where there is a need to train more doctors within the existing and limited hospital facilities. We have shown that immersive VR simulation is an accessible and scalable form of teaching life-threatening emergency scenarios which are difficult to access in a clinical setting. Unlike in-person simulation, VR simulations can be delivered to 30 to 60 students simultaneously with just one or two facilitators. This compares favourably to in-person simulation which requires one to two facilitators for up to four students, often with other students watching and critiquing rather than taking part themselves. Virtual reality headset costs may be a perceived barrier to implementation. However, good VR headsets (£300) compare favourably to the cost of desktop computers and iPads and markedly less than simulation suites (£100 k to 1 million). They do not incur the repeating costs of room and timetabling spaces, facilitators, and actors, thereby providing a cost-effective and re-usable method of teaching. VR simulation is therefore a scalable opportunity to educate and train more students effectively where space, time, and money is at a premium. However, we appreciate that in-person simulation also allows training in technical skills. Whilst not available with these 360-degree VR simulations, this is currently an evolving field in VR development with advancements in haptic feedback.

Given students experienced the VR and desktop scenarios on separate occasions, there was potential for contamination between groups with students relaying their experience of one format to a group that had not yet experienced it. Students were expected to account for their own personal experience. We did not ask about prior exposure to VR technology and acknowledge that a novelty effect may have influenced the result in favour of VR. There were no reports of kinetosis, hearing loss, or anxiety, but it is important to screen for these prospectively in future studies. This study did not evaluate knowledge retention, which would be difficult to quantify in this setting as it requires re-testing of students after they qualify as doctors. This would bring in numerous additional variables including re-recruitment difficulty and variation in postgraduate exposure to emergency scenarios between students. Most simulation in medicine is in-person simulation. This study does not compare VR simulation to in-person simulation as this comparison would require external eye tracking or tracking through augmented reality devices. It would be influenced by movement around the room which affects both eye movement and heart rate, whereas students physically move very little during VR scenarios. These VR scenarios are not a replacement for in-person simulation but allow a greater number of students to participate in an immersive experience, alongside exposure to a wider breadth of emergency scenarios. We plan to compare our VR simulation to in-person simulation in the future. Given the difficulties in monitoring physiological parameters for in-person simulation, we plan to use a qualitative approach to evaluate subjective engagement and enjoyment between the two arms of the study.

Conclusions

Our study has shown that VR simulations are enjoyable and more engaging compared to standard desktop simulation. The findings suggest that 360-degree VR simulation provides immersive and realistic training allowing students to manage emergencies that they may not experience during traditional training. VR-based learning blended with in-person clinical attachments may represent the future of health education, especially for clinical experiences which cannot be guaranteed in real life yet require situational awareness and the ability to remain level-headed in dynamic real-life scenarios.

Availability of data and materials

We commit to making the relevant anonymised data available on reasonable request. All data collected will be made available immediately following publication for 36 months to researchers who provide a methodologically sound proposal. This data will be available to achieve aims in the approved proposal. All proposals should be directed to the corresponding author.

Abbreviations

AOI:

Areas of interest

BERI:

Behavioural engagement related to instruction

HR:

Heart rate

ICSM:

Imperial College School of Medicine

PFA:

Pre-Foundation Assistantship

SBME:

Simulation-based medical education

VR:

Virtual reality

References

  1. Issenberg SB, McGaghie WC, Petrusa ER, Gordon DL, Scalese RJ. Features and uses of high-fidelity medical simulations that lead to effective learning: a BEME systematic review. Med Teach. 2005;27(1):10–28 Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16147767/ .

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Philippon AL, Truchot J, De Suremain N, Renaud MC, Petit A, Baron GL, et al. Medical students’ perception of simulation-based assessment in emergency and paediatric medicine: a focus group study. BMC Med Educ. 2021;21(1):1–12. Available from: https://bmcmededuc.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s12909-021-02957-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Cook DA, Andersen DK, Combes JR, Feldman DL, Sachdeva AK. The value proposition of simulation-based education. Surgery. 2018;163(4):944–9. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/29452702/.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Plotzky C, Lindwedel U, Sorber M, Loessl B, König P, Kunze C, et al. Virtual reality simulations in nurse education: a systematic mapping review. Nurse Educ Today. 2021;101. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33798987/.

  5. Petrica A, Lungeanu D, Ciuta A, Marza AM, Botea MO, Mederle OA. Using 360-degree video for teaching emergency medicine during and beyond the COVID-19 pandemic. Ann Med. 2021;53(1):1520–30 Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/34612105/ .

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Foronda CL, Shubeck K, Swoboda SM, Hudson KW, Budhathoki C, Sullivan N, et al. Impact of virtual simulation to teach concepts of disaster triage. Clin Simul Nurs. 2016;12(4):137–44 Available from: https://pure.johnshopkins.edu/en/publications/impact-of-virtual-simulation-to-teach-concepts-of-disaster-triage .

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Jennett C, Cox AL, Cairns P, Dhoparee S, Epps A, Tijs T, et al. Measuring and defining the experience of immersion in games. International Journal of Human Computer Studies. 2008;66(9):641–61. Available from: https://pure.york.ac.uk/portal/en/publications/measuring-and-defining-the-experience-of-immersion-in-games.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Hamilton J, Smith E, McWilliams G, Schwartz E, Carey J. Virtual reality: how a computer-generated world could change the real world. Bus Week. 1992;5:97–105.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Servotte JC, Goosse M, Campbell SH, Dardenne N, Pilote B, Simoneau IL, et al. Virtual reality experience: immersion, sense of presence, and cybersickness. Clin Simul Nurs. 2020;38:35–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Ulrich D, Farra S, Smith S, Hodgson E. The student experience using virtual reality simulation to teach decontamination. Clin Simul Nurs. 2014;10(11):546–53. Available from: https://www.academia.edu/19151649/The_Student_Experience_Using_Virtual_Reality_Simulation_to_Teach_Decontamination.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Jensen L, Konradsen F. A review of the use of virtual reality head-mounted displays in education and training. Educ Inf Technol (Dordr). 2018;23(4):1515–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Levin-Banchik L. Assessing knowledge retention, with and without simulations. Journal of Political Science Education. 2018;14(3):341–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Vadnais MA, Dodge LE, Awtrey CS, Ricciotti HA, Golen TH, Hacker MR. Assessment of long-term knowledge retention following single-day simulation training for uncommon but critical obstetrical events. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med. 2012;25(9):1640–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Akhu-Zaheya LM, Gharaibeh MK, Alostaz ZM. Effectiveness of simulation on knowledge acquisition, knowledge retention, and self-efficacy of nursing students in Jordan. Clin Simul Nurs. 2013;9(9):e335–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Lei H, Cui Y, Zhou W. Relationships between student engagement and academic achievement: a meta-analysis. Soc Behav Pers. 2018;46(3):517–28. Available from: https://www.sbp-journal.com/index.php/sbp/article/view/7054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. GuNuc S. The relationship between student engagement and their academic achievement. International Journal on New Trends in Education and Their Implications. 2014;5(4):216–31. Available from: http://www.sciepub.com/reference/230109.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Stephenson CR, Bonnes SL, Sawatsky AP, Richards LW, Schleck CD, Mandrekar JN, et al. The relationship between learner engagement and teaching effectiveness: a novel assessment of student engagement in continuing medical education. BMC Med Educ. 2020;20(1):1–8. Available from: https://bmcmededuc.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s12909-020-02331-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Greene BA. Measuring cognitive engagement with self-report scales: reflections from over 20 years of research. 2015;50(1):14–30. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2014.989230.

  19. Bligh DA. What’s the use of lectures? San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers; 1998.

  20. Darnell DK, Krieg PA. Student engagement, assessed using heart rate, shows no reset following active learning sessions in lectures. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(12):e0225709.

  21. Mahanama B, Jayawardana Y, Rengarajan S, Jayawardena G, Chukoskie L, Snider J, et al. Eye movement and pupil measures: a review. Front Comput Sci. 2022;3:1–22 Available from: https://digitalcommons.odu.edu/computerscience_fac_pubs/203 .

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Lane E, Harris S. A new tool for measuring student behavioral engagement in large university classes. J Coll Sci Teach. 2015;44(6):83–91. Available from: https://www.jstor.org/stable/43632000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Salvucci DD, Goldberg JH. Identifying fixations and saccades in eye-tracking protocols. In: Proceedings of the eye tracking research and applications symposium 2000. 2000. p. 71–8 Available from: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/220811146_Identifying_fixations_and_saccades_in_eye-tracking_protocols .

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  24. Rayner K. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychol Bull. 1998;124(3):372–422.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Salthouse TA, Ellis CL. Determinants of eye-fixation duration. Am J Psychol. 1980;93(2):207–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Holmqvist K, Nyström M, Andersson R, Dewhurst R, Halszka J, Weijer J van de. Eye tracking: a comprehensive guide to methods and measures. Oxford University Press; 2011. Available from: https://portal.research.lu.se/en/publications/eye-tracking-a-comprehensive-guide-to-methods-and-measures.

  27. Guo PJ, Kim J, Rubin R. How video production affects student engagement: an empirical study of MOOC videos. In: L@S 2014 - Proceedings of the 1st ACM Conference on Learning at Scale. 2014. p. 41–50 Available from: https://dl.acm.org/doi/10.1145/2556325.2566239 .

    Google Scholar 

  28. Kahneman D, Tursky B, Shapiro D, Crider A. Pupillary, heart rate, and skin resistance changes during a mental task. J Exp Psychol. 1969;79(1):164–7. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/5785627/.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Scholey AB, Moss MC, Neave N, Wesnes K. Cognitive performance, hyperoxia, and heart rate following oxygen administration in healthy young adults. Physiol Behav. 1999;67(5):783–9. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10604851/.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Cranford KN, Tiettmeyer JM, Chuprinko BC, Jordan S, Grove NP. Measuring load on working memory: the use of heart rate as a means of measuring chemistry students cognitive load. J Chem Educ. 2014;91(5):641–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Fredericks TK, Choi SD, Hart J, Butt SE, Mital A. An investigation of myocardial aerobic capacity as a measure of both physical and cognitive workloads. Int J Ind Ergon. 2005;35(12):1097–107. Available from: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/223866720_An_investigation_of_myocardial_aerobic_capacity_as_a_measure_of_both_physical_and_cognitive_workloads.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Expósito A, Sánchez-Rivas J, Gómez-Calero MP, Pablo-Romero MP. Examining the use of instructional video clips for teaching macroeconomics. Comput Educ. 2020;144:103709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Chen CM, Wang HP. Using emotion recognition technology to assess the effects of different multimedia materials on learning emotion and performance. Libr Inf Sci Res. 2011;33(3):244–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Thomas Shawcroft (senior camera operator), Penn Davis (3D artist), Catalina Ganea (producer), Charlie Graham (camera assistant), Giampaolo Trojani (senior video editor), Nikos Mexis (digital media facilities and solutions lead), Celia Brown (statistician), Ali Abbara (clinical senior lecturer), Tayo Olarewaju (producer), Argita Zalli (learning design lead), Lisa Carrier (Head of Technology Enhanced Education), Agata Sadza (Head of Digital Education Office), Tom Yates (information governance manager), Phill Doulton with professional role players (United Kingdom). TSh, PD, CGa, CGr, GT, and NM contributed to the production of the scenarios. CB and TO contributed to the planning of the study design. AZ helped with the conduct of the data collection session. LC, AA, and AgS contributed to planning the session, and TY helped with information governance. Professional role players provided actors for the production of the scenarios.

Funding

There was no study sponsor. Funding was from Imperial College London’s Digital Innovation Fund and from Health Education England Simulation Training Fund. We confirm the independence of researchers from funders.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RW and PN as joint first authors were instrumental in the planning, conducting, and reporting of the study through the study design, ethical approval, data collection, data analysis, write up, and editing of the manuscript. AC, TSe, TW, and SS contributed to the planning, conducting, and reporting of the study, through ethical approval, study design, data collection, and analysis. JM and PB contributed to the reporting of the study through data analysis, write up, and editing of the manuscript. DM contributed to the planning and conducting through the study design and cross-industry VR development, and AS contributed to the planning and reporting of the study through the study design, ethical approval, data analysis, write-up, and editing of the manuscript. All authors were involved in the drafting or revising the article. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. AS is the guarantor for the study. The guarantor accepts full responsibility for the work and/or the conduct of the study, had access to the data, and controlled the decision to publish. All authors confirm that they had full access to all the data in the study and accept responsibility to submit for publication. The corresponding author attests that all listed authors meet authorship criteria and that no others meeting the criteria have been omitted.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amir H. Sam.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Imperial College London Education Ethics Review Process (study ref. EERP2122-038) approved the study on 24 January 2022.

Consent for publication

Participants gave informed consent before taking part.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

12916_2024_3433_MOESM1_ESM.docx

Additional file 1. Images S1-S2. Image S1-Screenshot of the cardiac arrest scenario, created with 360-degree filming of an acted-out scenario. Image S2-Screenshot of the life-threatening asthma scenario, created with 360-degree filming of an acted-out scenario.

Additional file 2. Table S1. Table S1-Comparison of baseline data between standard desktop and VR groups.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Walls, R., Nageswaran, P., Cowell, A. et al. Virtual reality as an engaging and enjoyable method for delivering emergency clinical simulation training: a prospective, interventional study of medical undergraduates. BMC Med 22, 222 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-024-03433-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-024-03433-9

Keywords