Skip to main content

Differentiation between fetal and postnatal iron deficiency in altering brain substrates of cognitive control in pre-adolescence

Abstract

Background

Early iron deficiency (ID) is a common risk factor for poorer neurodevelopment, limiting children’s potential and contributing to global burden. However, it is unclear how early ID alters the substrate of brain functions supporting high-order cognitive abilities and whether the timing of early ID matters in terms of long-term brain development. This study aimed to examine the effects of ID during fetal or early postnatal periods on brain activities supporting proactive and reactive cognitive control in pre-adolescent children.

Methods

Participants were part of a longitudinal cohort enrolled at birth in southeastern China between December 2008 and November 2011. Between July 2019 and October 2021, 115 children aged 8–11 years were invited to participate in this neuroimaging study. Final analyses included 71 children: 20 with fetal ID, 24 with ID at 9 months (postnatal ID), and 27 iron-sufficient at birth and 9 months. Participants performed a computer-based behavioral task in a Magnetic Resonance Imaging scanner to measure proactive and reactive cognitive control. Outcome measures included accuracy, reaction times, and brain activity. Linear mixed modeling and the 3dlme command in Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) were separately used to analyze behavioral performance and neuroimaging data.

Results

Faster responses in proactive vs. reactive conditions indicated that all groups could use proactive or reactive cognitive control according to contextual demands. However, the fetal ID group was lower in general accuracy than the other 2 groups. Per the demands of cues and targets, the iron-sufficient group showed greater activation of wide brain regions in proactive vs. reactive conditions. In contrast, such condition differences were reversed in the postnatal ID group. Condition differences in brain activation, shown in postnatal ID and iron-sufficient groups, were not found in the fetal ID group. This group specifically showed greater activation of brain regions in the reward pathway in proactive vs. reactive conditions.

Conclusions

Early ID was associated with altered brain functions supporting proactive and reactive cognitive control in childhood. Alterations differed between fetal and postnatal ID groups. The findings imply that iron supplement alone is insufficient to prevent persisting brain alterations associated with early ID. Intervention strategies in addition to the iron supplement should consider ID timing.

Peer Review reports

Background

Anemia, mostly caused by iron deficiency (ID), is prevalent worldwide [1, 2]. ID is still a leading risk factor for those aged 0–24 years, contributing to global burden [3], particularly in developing countries [1, 2]. Iron is essential for normal brain development, especially during fetal and early postnatal life, which are critical/sensitive periods of brain development [4,5,6]. Animal studies indicate that early ID alters brain structures and functions related to synaptogenesis, synaptic plasticity, myelination, and neurotransmitters, particularly in hippocampal and striatal/basal ganglia areas [7,8,9,10]. In humans, electrophysiological and behavioral studies suggest that even with iron treatment or supplementation, early ID is associated with long-lasting impairments in the development of attention, memory, and motor skills based on prefrontal, hippocampal, or sensorimotor brain regions [4, 11,12,13,14]. Such neurocognitive developmental processes underlie the late onset and maturation of high-order cognitive functions [15].

To understand developmental adaptation and neuroplasticity associated with early ID, it is important to investigate neural mechanisms underlying the long-term effects on the development of high-order cognitive functions. To date, only two studies used functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine long-term effects of early ID on human brain functions. The results showed that ID anemia in infancy altered the co-activation of brain regions during the resting state in adulthood [16, 17]. Although these two preliminary studies are ground-breaking, there are limitations. First, information on iron status at birth was not available, meaning that the two studies could not consider the timing of early ID. Additionally, these two studies measured brain functions during the resting state, which lacks specificity in terms of high-order cognitive functions. Finally, Since magnetic resonance imaging methodologies were not feasible in the study context until participants were young adults, the study also could not offer insight into ID-associated changes in childhood or adolescence — which are sensitive periods for the development of high-order cognitive functions, such as cognitive control [18]. To fill these gaps, we applied task-based functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging methodologies in our longitudinal cohort to examine brain mechanisms underlying the long-term effects of fetal ID and postnatal ID on cognitive control in pre-adolescent children.

Alteration in cognitive control is associated with many developmental disorders (e.g., attention deficit hyperactivity disorder) [19]. Cognitive control — a high-order cognitive function — refers to the regulation of behaviors and thoughts in accordance with internally maintained goals [20]. According to the dual mechanisms of the control framework, cognitive control operates via two distinct operating modes, proactive control and reactive control [21]. The proactive control promotes the configuration of neurocognitive systems in advance according to internally maintained goals, whereas the reactive control refers to the stimulus-triggered just-in-time activation of brain systems to facilitate task processing [21]. As children get older, they gradually transition from mainly using reactive control to recruiting proactive control flexibly [22,23,24]. The flexible use of cognitive control depends on multiple brain networks (e.g., fronto-parietal and midcingulo-opercular networks) [25, 26] that do not reach maturity at least until late adolescence [27]. The long-term development of these brain networks is based on the brain circuits formed in early life [27]. As early brain development requires adequate iron supply [4], early ID may exert detrimental influences on brain development. Such influences may vary depending on early ID timing as the brain develops very rapidly in early life [28,29,30]. Therefore, we predicted that fetal ID and postnatal ID would both reshape brain development but in different ways, contributing to long-lasting alterations of brain functions that support the flexible use of cognitive control in pre-adolescent children.

To test the prediction, we recalled participants aged 8–11 years from a birth cohort established in southeastern China with documented iron status since birth. Then, we used task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging to collect brain activity data while participants were performing the cognitive control task in the scanner. This task includes proactive and reactive conditions. Compared to the non-informative cues in the reactive condition, the informative cues in the proactive condition can induce proactive control, enabling the recruitment of brain resources to select response rules before the onset of targets. Such selection in advance can support faster and more accurate responses to targets. We hypothesized that there were differences between the fetal ID, postnatal ID, and control groups in recruiting brain resources to support the cognitive processing in proactive vs. reactive conditions.

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited by the Brain and Behavior in Early Iron Deficiency study, a collaboration between the University of Michigan and the Children’s Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine. This was a longitudinal cohort project that aimed to evaluate the impact of early ID on neurodevelopment. Healthy full-term infants were enrolled at birth between December 2008 and November 2011 at the Fuyang Maternal and Children’s Health Care Hospital in southeastern China [31]. The infants enrolled in the Brain and Behavior in Early Iron Deficiency study met the following entrance criteria [31]: singleton term, birth weight ≥ 2500 g, 5-min Apgar score ≥ 7, and no delivery complications, maternal or infant health problems, or major congenital deficits. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Michigan and the Children’s Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine. The demographic information, such as parental age, parental education, parental occupation, and household income, was collected by qualified project personnel when the cohort study was set up (i.e., at 6-week-old and 9-month-old). The family socioeconomic status was assessed by the combination of parental education, parental occupation, and household income (Additional file 1: The computation of family socioeconomic status).

Between October 2019 and October 2021, 115 children aged 8–11 years and their families were invited back to participate in this study. Nineteen children declined to go into the scanner or did not finish the test. Another 25 children were excluded for the following reasons: technical problems (2), low behavioral performance (2), health reasons (1), or too much head motion [20]. After the attrition, 71 participants were included in analyses (mean age = 10.0 years, SD = 0.9, 37 males, 34 females). This Zhejiang University follow-up study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Children’s Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine. Before participating in this study, parents and children separately signed consent and assent forms.

Iron status

Iron status was assessed at birth using cord blood and repeated at 9 months, 18 months, and 8–11 years old using venous blood. Measures of iron status included serum ferritin (SF), zinc protoporphyrin/heme ratio (ZPP/H), mean corpuscular volume (MCV), and/or red cell distribution width (RDW). Hemoglobin (Hb) was used to assess anemia status. Serum C-reactive protein was measured to exclude inflammation that may interfere with the diagnosis of iron status. The iron assay methods were documented in our previous publications [30,31,32]. Fetal ID was defined as cord SF < 75 µg/l or ZPP/H > 118 µmol/mol [33,34,35]; postnatal ID was defined as normal cord iron status at birth and ≥ 2 abnormal iron measures at 9 months using the following cutoffs: MCV < 74 fl [36], RDW > 14.5% [37], SF < 12.0 µg/l [38], and ZPP/H > 69 µmol/mol [39]; iron sufficiency refers to no ID both at birth and 9 months. Infants with cord SF < 60 µg/L randomly received the iron supplement or placebo according to the design of a small randomized controlled trial [32]. All children with ID anemia at 9 months received iron therapy. In the sample for functional magnetic resonance imaging analyses, there were 20 children with fetal ID, 24 children with postnatal ID, and 27 children with iron sufficiency in early life. Among children with fetal ID, 8 children had ID only at birth, and 12 children had ID both at birth and 9 months. No participant had ID or ID anemia at 18 months and 8–11 years. The 3 groups did not differ in sex, age, head motion, parental age, parental education, parental occupation, household income, and family socioeconomic status (ps > 0.10). More details are provided in Additional file 1.

Behavioral task

Participants performed a widely used computer-based behavioral task [22, 40] in the Magnetic Resonance Imaging scanner to measure proactive and reactive cognitive control (Fig. 1, Additional file 1: Protocol of the behavioral task). It included 2 sessions with background cartoon characters of Winnie or Donald. In each session, 2 proactive blocks and 2 reactive blocks were administrated randomly. In proactive blocks, a colored border was presented with one cartoon character as an informative cue, followed by a target picture of food, animal, or objects. In reactive blocks, black borders were presented with cartoon characters as non-informative cues, and colored borders were not presented until the onset of target pictures. Colored borders and cartoon characters jointly determined response rules. According to such rules, participants were asked to respond to target pictures by pressing buttons using the left or right index finger. Thus, proactive blocks allowed participants to figure out the rule of a trial before the onset of targets, but the rule in reactive blocks could not be identified until the onset of targets. There were 20 target pictures in each block, which contained switch and repeat conditions and each condition had 8–11 trials. For switch trials, rules in current trials were changed compared to previous trials, whereas there was no such change in repeat trials. Block duration was 2 min and 13 s with a 17-s break between blocks. Stimuli positions and response rules were counterbalanced across participants.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Design of the cued task-switching paradigm. To counterbalance stimuli positions and response rules across participants, we created 4 versions of this task. Each participant was randomly assigned to one version (one example version was presented above). In each version, there were Winnie and Donald sessions. In each session, there were 80 trials, and each trial included the periods of Fixation, Cue, Interval, and Target. In proactive blocks, the border was green or red during the Cue period, whereas such colored border was not presented until the presence of targets in the reactive block. Participants were asked to make responses in Target periods by pressing the left or right key in each trial. Response rules were jointly determined by the cartoon character and colored border and differed between Winnie and Donald sessions. For example, in the Winnie session, the green border indicated that the rule was to judge whether the target was a food picture (i.e., green-food rule), and the red border was to judge whether the target was an animal picture (i.e., red-animal rule). Compared to the Winnie session, rules in the Donald session were reversed (i.e., red-food and green-animal). Such task rules were counterbalanced across participants

Imaging data acquisition and preprocessing

Participants were scanned in a Siemens 3.0-T scanner (MAGNETOM Prisma, Siemens Healthcare Erlangen, Germany) using a 60-channel coil. Before formal scanning, children learned how to stay still in a mock scanner. Then, participants completed a series of structural and functional scans in the real scanner. The structural images were acquired with a T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo sequence: TR (repetition time) = 2300 ms, TE (echo time) = 2.32 ms, slice thickness = 0.9 mm, voxel size = 0.90 × 0.90 × 0.90 mm3, voxel matrix = 256 × 256, flip angle = 8°, field of view = 240 mm. During task scanning, a total of 546 whole-brain volumes were collected using a T2-weighted gradient echo planar imaging sequence with multi-bands acceleration (TR = 1000 ms, TE = 34 ms, slice thickness = 2.50 mm, voxel size = 2.50 × 2.50 × 2.50 mm3, voxel matrix = 92 × 92, flip angle = 50°, field of view = 230 mm2, slice number = 52, MB-factor = 4).

Preprocessing neuroimaging data consisted of several steps. First, slice timing and head motion were corrected using AFNI (Analysis of Functional NeuroImages, Cox, 1996). Then, tissue segmentation was conducted to extract brains using SPM12 (Statistical Parametric Mapping 12, https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Structural and functional images were normalized to the MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) space using ANTs (Advanced Normalization Tools, http://stnava.github.io/ANTs/). Finally, spatial smoothing was conducted with a 5-mm full-width-at-half-maximum Gaussian kernel. For first-level analyses, multiple regression analyses were conducted using the 3dDeconvolve command within AFNI. Events during encoding were convolved with the hemodynamic function to create a total of 8 regressors of interest (2 sessions: session 1 vs. 2; 2 control conditions: proactive vs. reactive; 2 switch conditions: repeat vs. switch) for cue- and target-elicited brain activity. Six head motion parameters were included as covariates. To alleviate the impact of head motion, we censored the volumes with framewise displacement > 0.5 mm. All subjects included for statistical analyses had mean framewise displacement (FD) within 0.13 to 0.44 mm.

Statistical analyses

Linear mixed modeling (IBM SPSS Statistics 22, IBM Corp., Chicago, IL, USA) was used to analyze behavioral performance as indexed by reaction times and accuracy. In fixed-effect models, Group, Control, Switch, and their interactions were included as predictors. The subject factor was included in the random-effect model. We compared the models that contained only main effects to the ones that also included interactions. We selected the best-fitting models with the lowest Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC) values. The selected models and their AIC values were presented in Additional file 1. For neuroimaging data, the 3dlme command in AFNI was used to test the main effects of Group, Control, Switch, and their interactions in predicting cue- and target-elicited brain activity. The 3dClustSim mixed model autocorrelation function indicated that clusters with uncorrected p < 0.001 and a minimum of 27 voxel size remained significant after multiple comparison corrections (pcorrected < 0.05). Such correction was carried out for all reported neuroimaging results. The results below focus on differences between groups in behavioral performance and brain activity.

Results

Behavioral performance

Children in all groups showed faster responses in proactive vs. reactive conditions (β =  − 428.0; 95% CI, − 483.5 to − 372.4; p < 0.001), but there was no significant difference in reaction times between groups. For accuracy, no group showed statistically significant differences between proactive and reactive conditions. The fetal ID group was lower in overall accuracy than both the postnatal ID (β =  − 0.06; 95% CI, − 0.10 to − 0.01; p = 0.02) and iron-sufficient groups (β =  − 0.08; 95% CI, − 0.12 to − 0.04; p = 0.001). More details are provided in Additional file 1: Table S4.

Neuroimaging results

Consistent with behavioral results, all groups showed significant differences in cue- and target-elicited brain activity between proactive and reactive conditions (Fig. 2; Additional file 1: Table S5-S6). Additionally, such condition differences in brain activity varied between groups, as evidenced by significant Group × Control interactions at 6 clusters for cue-elicited brain activity (Table 1, Fig. 3) and at 21 clusters for target-elicited brain activity (Table 2, Fig. 4).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Common brain regions recruited by three groups to process cues (A) and targets (B) in proactive vs. reactive conditions. Orange patches denote common brain regions showing greater activation or weaker deactivation to process cues or targets in proactive vs. reactive conditions, while common brain regions showing reversed condition differences are represented by blue patches. The t values denote differences between proactive and reactive conditions. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; Amy, amygdala; AnG, angular gyrus; CCG, calcarine gyrus; Cun, cuneus; dStr, dorsal striatum; FuG, fusiform gyrus; Hip, hippocampus; IFGor, inferior frontal gyrus (p. orbitalis); IFGop, inferior frontal gyrus (p. opercularis); IFGtr, inferior frontal gyrus (p. triangularis); InL, insula; IocG, inferior occipital gyrus; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; LG, lingual gyrus; MCC, middle cingulate cortex; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; MocG, middle occipital gyrus; MorG, mid orbital gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; Hip, hippocampus; OlfC, olfactory cortex; ParCL, paracentral lobe; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; PoG, postcentral gyrus; PrG, precentral gyrus; Pcun, precuneus; RO, rolandic operculum; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; SMA, supplemental motor area; SmeG, superior medial gyrus; SMG, supramarginal gyrus; SocG, superior occipital gyrus; SorG, superior orbital gyrus; SPL, superior parietal lobe; STG, superior temporal gyrus; Tha, thalamus; TP, temporal pole

Table 1 Brain regions showing significant Control × Group interaction in cue-elicited activity
Fig. 3
figure 3

Specific brain regions recruited by each group to process cues in proactive vs. reactive conditions. In each group, orange patches denote regions showing greater activation or weaker deactivation to process cues in proactive vs. reactive conditions, while blue patches denote regions showing reversed conditional differences. The t values denote differences between proactive and reactive conditions. dStr, dorsal striatum; HesG, heschl’s gyrus; InL, insula; PHG, parahippocampus; RO, rolandic operculum; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; SMA, supplemental motor area; SorG, superior orbital gyrus; STG, superior temporal gyrus

Table 2 Brain regions showing significant Control × Group interaction in target-elicited activity
Fig. 4
figure 4

Specific brain regions recruited by each group to process targets in proactive vs. reactive conditions. In each group, orange patches denote regions showing greater activation or weaker deactivation to process targets in proactive vs. reactive conditions, while blue patches denote regions showing reversed conditional differences. The t values denote differences between proactive and reactive conditions. AnG, angular gyrus; Cau, caudate nucleus; Cun, cuneus; HesG, heschl’s gyrus; MCC: middle cingulate cortex; FuG, fusiform gyrus; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; MocG, middle occipital gyrus; MorG, mid orbital gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; IFGor, inferior frontal gyrus (p. orbitalis); IFGtr, inferior frontal gyrus (p. triangularis); InL, insula; IPL, inferior parietal lobe; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; ParCL, paracentral lobe; Pcun, precuneus; PoG, postcentral gyrus; RO, rolandic operculum; SMA, supplemental motor area; SmeG, superior medial gyrus; SMG, supramarginal gyrus; SorG, superior orbital gyrus; STG, superior temporal gyrus; SPL, superior parietal lobe; TP, temporal pole

Cue-elicited brain activity

Three groups recruited common brain regions to process cues

Compared to the reactive condition, all groups showed greater activity in the proactive condition at the visual cortex, fronto-parietal, and subcortical regions, such as bilateral lingual gyrus, bilateral fusiform gyrus, right middle frontal gyrus, bilateral precentral gyrus, right superior/inferior parietal lobe, bilateral thalamus, and right dorsal striatum (see these brain regions in Fig. 2 and in Additional file 1: Table S5). Additionally, compared to the proactive condition, all groups showed greater activity in the reactive condition at brain regions that are widely distributed, including the bilateral parts of the superior medial gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, postcentral gyrus, insula and operculum, cingulate cortex, hippocampus, precuneus, and occipital gyrus (see all brain regions in Fig. 2 and Additional file 1: Table S5).

Differences between groups in brain regions recruited to process cues

As shown in Table 1 and Fig. 3, the iron-sufficient group showed greater activity in reactive vs. proactive conditions at the left superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and left supplemental motor area (SMA) and showed greater activity in proactive vs. reactive conditions at the left caudate nucleus and left putamen. The postnatal ID group showed greater activity in reactive vs. proactive conditions at the bilateral superior temporal gyrus (STG), bilateral posterior insula, left posterior rolandic operculum (RO), left heschl’s gyrus (HesG), and right parahippocampus. The fetal ID group had greater activity in proactive vs. reactive conditions at the left superior orbital gyrus (SorG) and left putamen.

Target-elicited brain activity

Three groups recruited common brain regions to process targets

All groups showed greater activity at the bilateral insula and operculum, left supramarginal gyrus, left olfactory cortex, left amygdala, and left hippocampus in proactive vs. reactive conditions (see all brain regions in Fig. 2 and in Additional file 1: Table S6). In addition, compared to the proactive condition, all groups showed greater activity in the reactive condition at frontal, occipital-temporal, subcortical, and motor regions, such as the right superior frontal gyrus, right anterior cingulate cortex, as well as the bilateral parts of the inferior frontal gyrus, calcarine gyrus, superior occipital gyrus, lingual gyrus, fusiform gyrus, thalamus, dorsal striatum, and precentral gyrus (see all brain regions in Fig. 2 and Additional file 1: Table S6).

Differences between groups in brain regions recruited to process targets

As shown in Table 2 and Fig. 4, the iron-sufficient group showed greater activity in proactive vs. reactive conditions at the fronto-parietal network, temporal regions, limbic system, and insula, such as the orbital part of left inferior frontal gyrus (IFGor), bilateral posterior precuneus, bilateral middle cingulate cortex (MCC), right STG, and left insula (see all brain regions in Table 2). Such condition differences shown by the iron-sufficient group were reversed in the postnatal ID group, which showed greater activity in reactive vs. proactive conditions at widely-distributed brain regions, including the bilateral parts of the middle orbital gyrus (MorG), superior medial gyrus (SmeG), superior parietal lobule (SPL), precuneus, middle and inferior temporal gyrus, as well as left MCC, right middle occipital gyrus, and right caudate nucleus (see all brain regions in Table 2). The fetal ID group showed greater activity in proactive vs. reactive conditions only at the bilateral parts of SorG, MorG, and SmeG.

Discussion

This study found commonalities and differences in behavioral and neuroimaging results across the 3 groups defined by iron status in early life. Behavioral results indicated that children with or without early ID were able to use proactive and reactive cognitive control according to contextual demands. Due to different demands between proactive and reactive conditions, the cues and targets in these 2 conditions activated different brain regions, some of which were similar across groups. Specifically, all 3 groups showed greater cue-elicited activity in reactive vs. proactive conditions at brain regions from the fronto-parietal, sensorimotor, and cingulo-opercular networks, which are associated with maintaining response rules and monitoring incoming information [41,42,43]. In contrast, the cues in proactive vs. reactive conditions elicited greater activity in visual regions, such as the fusiform and lingual gyrus [44], associated with processing color information, as well as in the cortical-striatum-thalamus pathway, associated with selecting and updating rule representation [45]. Such brain activity elicited by cues in the proactive condition was not induced until targets arrived in the reactive condition. The targets in the proactive condition induced greater activity than that in the reactive condition in sensorimotor and cingulo-opercular networks associated with selecting and executing responses [46, 47]. In sum, to process the cues and targets in proactive vs. reactive conditions, three groups showed commonality in recruited brain regions.

Such similarities notwithstanding, the 3 groups differed in the activation of some brain regions. For the iron-sufficient group only, the cues in proactive vs. reactive conditions elicited greater activity at left dorsal striatum, associated with selecting or updating rule representation [48], whereas the cues in reactive vs. proactive conditions elicited greater activity at left SFG and SMA, associated with maintaining response rules [47, 49]. Only the iron-sufficient group made faster responses to the targets in proactive vs. reactive conditions by eliciting greater activity at several brain regions within the fronto-parietal and cingulo-opercular networks [43], such as IFGor, SMA, SPL, MCC, and insula. These findings indicate that, compared to the 2 ID groups, the iron-sufficient group recruited more brain resources to make faster responses in proactive vs. reactive conditions. Thus, consistent with animal studies [9, 50, 51], ID in the early life of human beings may impair the development of such brain networks as the fronto-parietal and cingulo-opercular networks. Such impairment in brain development might lead to the observed differences in the brain networks recruited to support cognitive control between pre-adolescent children with early ID and those who were iron-sufficient in infancy.

The postnatal ID group showed greater activity elicited by cues and targets in reactive vs. proactive conditions. Specifically, the cues in the reactive condition activated brain regions related to speech repetition, memory, and linguistic processing [52], such as bilateral posterior insula [53, 54], left RO [55], left HesG [56], bilateral STG [57], and right parahippocampus [58]. These results suggest that the postnatal ID group might have used verbal rehearsal strategies to maintain general task rules in the memory system during the cue period. To process the targets in reactive vs. proactive conditions, the postnatal ID group activated widely-distributed brain regions from the fronto-parietal [25, 42], cingulo-opercular [43, 59], sensorimotor [41], and temporal-occipital [60] networks, associated with processing visual stimuli, integrating multi-sensory information, selecting rules and responses, and making responses. Thus, in contrast to the other two groups, the postnatal ID group appeared to use strategies and extra brain resources to support cognitive control in reactive vs. proactive conditions, suggesting that children with postnatal ID might not be able to use informative cues to stimulate proactive control as well as iron-sufficient children, or the informative cues in the proactive condition might interfere with postnatal ID children’s use of strategies that were internally driven in the reactive condition.

The fetal ID group recruited the orbital gyrus and putamen to process the cues and targets in proactive vs. reactive conditions. These two brain regions are in the brain pathways responsible for processing rewards and monitoring outcomes [61]. Thus, the informative cues in the proactive condition appeared to activate the reward pathway in the fetal ID group. This interpretation is consistent with previous studies showing that early ID is associated with later alterations in sensitivity to reward and punishment [8, 14, 62, 63].

The observed group differences suggest that the alteration of brain networks supporting cognitive control in childhood relates to ID timing in early life. Such findings are consistent with previous studies [29, 30], which detected differences between fetal and postnatal ID in their influences on cognitive and motor development. Such differences related to ID timing might be due to the trajectories of brain development in early life. For example, the brain undergoes rapid development from the fetal to the early postnatal period through neurulation, neuronal proliferation, neural migration, apoptosis, synaptogenesis, and myelination [27]. These neural developmental processes might be differently affected by fetal vs. postnatal ID. Furthermore, due to developmental adaptation and neuroplasticity, children with early ID at different times might have developed different compensatory brain networks or paths to support cognitive control in childhood [64].

Strengths and limitations

This study could not address how fetal and postnatal ID jointly affect cognitive and brain development as the sample size did not allow us to create a group with ID at both fetal and postnatal periods. Additionally, it would be more informative if neuroimaging data were collected at more time points in addition to the one at 10-year-old. Despite these limitations, this study is based on a longitudinal project that has followed participants for about 10 years. The data are not only rare but also provide important insight into the long-term effects of early ID on brain development.

Conclusions

Despite the iron supplement in infancy, early ID was associated with altered brain activity during cognitive control about 10 years later. The altered brain functions differed between children with fetal and postnatal ID. These findings support the need to prevent ID during critical/sensitive periods of brain development. This is especially important considering the persistent prevalence of ID in pregnant women and infants [2]. Additionally, the findings suggest that iron treatment alone cannot entirely prevent the long-lasting alterations of brain functions related to early ID. Developing precise intervention strategies for early ID should consider ID timing. Future studies need to investigate whether the altered brain correlates of cognitive control underlie the associations between early ID and long-term impaired development in both socioemotional and cognitive functioning.

Availability of data and materials

The de-identified datasets generated and analyzed in the current study are available at https://doi.org/10.57760/sciencedb.07780.

Abbreviations

AFNI:

Analysis of Functional NeuroImages

AIC:

Akaike’s Information Criterion

ANTs:

Advanced normalization tools

CI:

Confidence interval

FD:

Framewise displacement

Hb:

Hemoglobin

HesG:

Heschl’s gyrus

ID:

Iron deficiency

IFGor:

Inferior frontal gyrus (p. orbitalis)

MCC:

Middle cingulate cortex

MCV:

Mean corpuscular volume

MNI:

Montreal neurological institute

MorG:

Mid orbital gyrus

RDW:

Red cell distribution width

RO:

Rolandic operculum

SF:

Serum ferritin

SFG:

Superior frontal gyrus

SMA:

Supplemental motor area

SmeG:

Superior medial gyrus

SorG:

Superior orbital gyrus

SPL:

Superior parietal lobe

SPM:

Statistical parametric mapping

STG:

Superior temporal gyrus

TE:

Echo time

TR:

Repetition time

ZPP/H:

Zinc protoporphyrin/heme ratio

References

  1. Stevens GA, Finucane MM, De-Regil LM, Paciorek CJ, Flaxman SR, Branca F, et al. Global, regional, and national trends in haemoglobin concentration and prevalence of total and severe anaemia in children and pregnant and non-pregnant women for 1995–2011: a systematic analysis of population-representative data. Lancet Glob Health. 2013;1(1):e16–25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. WHO. The global prevalence of anaemia in 2011. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Murray CJL, Aravkin AY, Zheng P, Abbafati C, Abbas KM, Abbasi-Kangevari M, et al. Global burden of 87 risk factors in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet. 2020;396(10258):1223–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. McCann S, Perapoch Amadó M, Moore SE. The role of iron in brain development: a systematic review. Nutrients. 2020;12(7):2001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Thompson DK, Matthews LG, Alexander B, Lee KJ, Kelly CE, Adamson CL, et al. Tracking regional brain growth up to age 13 in children born term and very preterm. Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):696.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Georgieff MK, Brunette KE, Tran PV. Early life nutrition and neural plasticity. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;27(2):411–23.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Markova V, Holm C, Pinborg A, Thomsen L, Moos T. Impairment of the developing human brain in iron deficiency: correlations to findings in experimental animals and prospects for early intervention therapy. Pharmaceuticals. 2019;12(3):120.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Lozoff B. Early iron deficiency has brain and behavior effects consistent with dopaminergic dysfunction. J Nutr. 2011;141(4):740s-s746.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Bastian TW, Von Hohenberg WC, Mickelson DJ, Lanier LM, Georgieff MK. Iron deficiency impairs developing hippocampal neuron gene expression, energy metabolism, and dendrite complexity. Dev Neurosci. 2016;38(4):264–76.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Lien YC, Condon DE, Georgieff MK, Simmons RA, Tran PV. Dysregulation of neuronal genes by fetal-neonatal iron deficiency anemia is associated with altered DNA methylation in the rat hippocampus. Nutrients. 2019;11(5):1191.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Lukowski AF, Koss M, Burden MJ, Jonides J, Nelson CA, Kaciroti N, et al. Iron deficiency in infancy and neurocognitive functioning at 19 years: evidence of long-term deficits in executive function and recognition memory. Nutr Neurosci. 2010;13(2):54–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Algarin C, Nelson CA, Peirano P, Westerlund A, Reyes S, Lozoff B. Iron-deficiency anemia in infancy and poorer cognitive inhibitory control at age 10 years. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(5):453–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Wolf AW. Long-term developmental outcome of infants with iron deficiency. N Engl J Med. 1991;325(10):687–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Hagen J, Mollen E, Wolf AW. Poorer behavioral and developmental outcome more than 10 years after treatment for iron deficiency in infancy. Pediatrics. 2000;105(4):e51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Cadwell CR, Bhaduri A, Mostajo-Radji MA, Keefe MG, Nowakowski TJ. Development and arealization of the cerebral cortex. Neuron. 2019;103(6):980–1004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Algarin C, Karunakaran KD, Reyes S, Morales C, Lozoff B, Peirano P, et al. Differences on brain connectivity in adulthood are present in subjects with iron deficiency anemia in infancy. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Cecilia A, Patricio P, Donna C, Rakibul H, Sussanne R, Betsy L, et al. Cognitive control inhibition networks in adulthood are impaired by early iron deficiency in infancy. Neuroimage Clin. 2022;35: 103089.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Sydnor VJ, Larsen B, Bassett DS, Alexander-Bloch A, Fair DA, Liston C, et al. Neurodevelopment of the association cortices: patterns, mechanisms, and implications for psychopathology. Neuron. 2021;109(18):2820–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Tajik-Parvinchi D, Farmus L, Tablon Modica P, Cribbie RA, Weiss JA. The role of cognitive control and emotion regulation in predicting mental health problems in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Child Care Health Dev. 2021;47(5):608–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Piray P, Daw ND. Linear reinforcement learning in planning, grid fields, and cognitive control. Nat Commun. 2021;12(1):4942.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Braver TS. The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(2):106–13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Chevalier N, Martis SB, Curran T, Munakata Y. Metacognitive processes in executive control development: the case of reactive and proactive control. J Cogn Neurosci. 2015;27(6):1125–36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Chatham CH, Frank MJ, Munakata Y. Pupillometric and behavioral markers of a developmental shift in the temporal dynamics of cognitive control. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106(14):5529–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Gonthier C, Zira M, Colé P, Blaye A. Evidencing the developmental shift from reactive to proactive control in early childhood and its relationship to working memory. J Exp Child Psychol. 2019;177:1–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Menon V, D’Esposito M. The role of PFC networks in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47(1):90–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Somerville LH, Casey BJ. Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010;20(2):236–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Tau GZ, Peterson BS. Normal development of brain circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):147–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Lozoff B, Georgieff MK. Iron deficiency and brain development. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2006;13(3):158–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Santos DCC, Angulo-Barroso RM, Li M, Bian Y, Sturza J, Richards B, et al. Timing, duration, and severity of iron deficiency in early development and motor outcomes at 9 months. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2018;72(3):332–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Geng F, Mai X, Zhan J, Xu L, Georgieff M, Shao J, et al. Timing of iron deficiency and recognition memory in infancy. Nutr Neurosci. 2020;25(1):1–10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Armony-Sivan R, Zhu B, Clark KM, Richards B, Ji C, Kaciroti N, et al. Iron deficiency (ID) at both birth and 9 months predicts right frontal EEG asymmetry in infancy. Dev Psychobiol. 2016;58(4):462–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Shao J, Richards B, Kaciroti N, Zhu B, Clark KM, Lozoff B. Contribution of iron status at birth to infant iron status at 9 months: data from a prospective maternal-infant birth cohort in China. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2021;75(2):364–72.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Amin SB, Orlando M, Eddins A, Macdonald M, Monczynski C, Wang H. In utero iron status and auditory neural maturation in premature infants as evaluated by auditory brainstem response. J Pediatr. 2010;156(3):377–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Tamura T, Goldenberg RL, Hou JR, Johnston KE, Cliver SP, Ramey SL, et al. Cord serum ferritin concentrations and mental and psychomotor development of children at five years of age. J Pediatr. 2002;140(2):165–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. McLimore HM, Phillips AK, Blohowiak SE, Pham DQD, Coe CL, Fischer BA, et al. Impact of multiple prenatal risk factors on newborn iron status at delivery. J Pediatr Hematol Oncol. 2013;35(6):473–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. NCHS. Healthy people 2000 final review. Maryland: Public Health Service; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  37. CDC. Recommendations to prevent and control iron deficiency in the United States. MMWR Recomm Rep. 1998;47(Rr-3):1–29.

  38. Saarinen UM, Siimes MA. Serum ferritin in assessment of iron nutrition in healthy infants. Acta Paediatr. 1978;67(6):745–51.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Porat Soldin O, Miller M, Soldin SJ. Pediatric reference ranges for zinc protoporphyrin. Clin Biochem. 2003;36(1):21–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Geng F, Redcay E, Riggins T. The influence of age and performance on hippocampal function and the encoding of contextual information in early childhood. Neuroimage. 2019;195:433–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Marvel CL, Morgan OP, Kronemer SI. How the motor system integrates with working memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;102:184–94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Ptak R, Schnider A, Fellrath J. The dorsal frontoparietal network: a core system for emulated action. Trends Cogn Sci. 2017;21(8):589–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Dosenbach NUF, Fair DA, Cohen AL, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12(3):99–105.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Simmons WK, Ramjee V, Beauchamp MS, McRae K, Martin A, Barsalou LW. A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(12):2802–10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Liljeholm M, O’Doherty JP. Contributions of the striatum to learning, motivation, and performance: an associative account. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(9):467–75.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Newbold DJ, Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Seider NA, Montez DF, Gross SJ, et al. Cingulo-opercular control network and disused motor circuits joined in standby mode. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2021;118(13):e2019128118.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Cona G, Semenza C. Supplementary motor area as key structure for domain-general sequence processing: a unified account. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;72:28–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Cools R. Dopaminergic control of the striatum for high-level cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2011;21(3):402–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Ott T, Nieder A. Dopamine and cognitive control in prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019;23(3):213–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Ward KL, Tkac I, Jing Y, Felt B, Beard J, Connor J, et al. Gestational and lactational iron deficiency alters the developing striatal metabolome and associated behaviors in young rats. J Nutr. 2007;137(4):1043–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Unger EL, Paul T, Murray-Kolb LE, Felt B, Jones BC, Beard JL. Early iron deficiency alters sensorimotor development and brain monoamines in rats. J Nutr. 2007;137(1):118–24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Saur D, Kreher BW, Schnell S, Kümmerer D, Kellmeyer P, Vry M-S, et al. Ventral and dorsal pathways for language. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105(46):18035–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Ghaziri J, Tucholka A, Girard G, Houde J-C, Boucher O, Gilbert G, et al. The corticocortical structural connectivity of the human insula. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27(2):1216–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Dronkers NF. A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation. Nature. 1996;384(6605):159–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Mălîia M-D, Donos C, Barborica A, Popa I, Ciurea J, Cinatti S, et al. Functional mapping and effective connectivity of the human operculum. Cortex. 2018;109:303–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Wong P, Warrier C, Penhune V, Roy A, Sadehh A, Parrish T, et al. Volume of left Heschl’s Gyrus and linguistic pitch learning. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(4):828–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Yi HG, Leonard MK, Chang EF. The encoding of speech sounds in the superior temporal gyrus. Neuron. 2019;102(6):1096–110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Van Strien NM, Cappaert NLM, Witter MP. The anatomy of memory: an interactive overview of the parahippocampal–hippocampal network. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(4):272–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Wallis G, Stokes M, Cousijn H, Woolrich M, Nobre AC. Frontoparietal and cingulo-opercular networks play dissociable roles in control of working memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2015;27(10):2019–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Roland P, Gulyás B. Visual imagery and visual representation. Trends Neurosci. 1994;17(7):281–7 discussion 94-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Haber SN, Knutson B. The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):4–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Lozoff B, Klein NK, Nelson EC, McClish DK, Manuel M, Chacon ME. Behavior of infants with iron-deficiency anemia. Child Dev. 1998;69(1):24–36.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Lozoff B, Castillo M, Clark KM, Smith JB, Sturza J. Iron supplementation in infancy contributes to more adaptive behavior at 10 years of age. J Nutr. 2014;144(6):838–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Graham AM, Marr M, Buss C, Sullivan EL, Fair DA. Understanding vulnerability and adaptation in early brain development using network neuroscience. Trends Neurosci. 2021;44(4):276–88.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the children and their families for participating in this study and Youjin Wu, Qiuping Ding, and other team members for assisting with the cohort maintenance, participant enrollment, and data collection.

Funding

This work is supported by the National Key Research and Development Program of China (2019YFC0840702), the National Natural Science Foundation of China (General Program; 81773440, 62077042), the MOE (Ministry of Education in China) Project of Humanities and Social Sciences (20YJA190002), the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities, and the Zhejiang University Education Foundation Global Partnership Fund.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MH, DS, and WX collected data, conducted analyses, as well as wrote and revised the manuscript. LZ, XH, and BZ collected and analyzed data and revised the manuscript. QS contributed to study conception and design as well as manuscript preparation and revision. BL, FG, and JS contributed to study conception, study design, data analyses, funding acquirement, as well as manuscript preparation and revision. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Fengji Geng or Jie Shao.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Brain and Behavior in Early Iron Deficiency study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Michigan and the Children’s Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine with Approval No. 2008004. The Zhejiang University follow-up study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Children’s Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine with Approval No. 2017-IRBAL-022. Parents provided signed consent and children provided informed assent before participating in this study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: 1. The protocol of the behavioral task. 2. The computation of family socioeconomic status. 3. The selection of best-fitting models. Table S1.

The demographics of each group. Table S2. Iron status at birth, 9 months, 18 months, and 8-to-11 years. Table S3. The AIC values of each model. Table S4. Behavioral results for each group and condition. Table S5. Common brain regions recruited by three groups to process cues in proactive vs. reactive conditions. Table S6. Common brain regions recruited by three groups to process targets in proactive vs. reactive conditions.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hua, M., Shi, D., Xu, W. et al. Differentiation between fetal and postnatal iron deficiency in altering brain substrates of cognitive control in pre-adolescence. BMC Med 21, 167 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-023-02850-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-023-02850-6

Keywords