Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Self- and interviewer-reported cognitive problems in relation to cognitive decline and dementia: results from two prospective studies

Abstract

Background

Little is known regarding the association of interviewer-reported cognitive problems (ICP) with age-related cognitive decline. We aimed to investigate the independent associations of ICP and the combined associations of ICP and self-reported cognitive problems (SCP) with subsequent cognitive decline and dementia in two prospective cohort studies.

Methods

We included 10,976 Chinese (age = 57.7 ± 8.7) and 40,499 European (age = 64.6 ± 9.4) adults without dementia from the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS) and the Survey of Health, Ageing, and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). Self-rated memory (5-point scale) and interviewer-rated frequencies of asking for clarification (6-point scale) were used to define SCP and ICP (dichotomized). Outcomes included objective cognitive test scores (z-score transformation) and incident dementia. Generalized estimating equation models were performed to evaluate mean differences in objective cognitive decline. Logistic and Cox regression models were used to estimate the relative risk of dementia. Results from two cohorts were pooled using the random-effects models.

Results

ICP was associated with faster cognitive decline in CHARLS (βCHARLS = −0.025 [−0.044, −0.006] z-score/year). ICP and SCP were also independently associated with higher risk of dementia in two cohorts (pooled relative risk for SCP = 1.73 [1.30, 2.29]; pooled relative risk for ICP = 1.40 [1.10, 1.79]). In the joint analysis, participants with coexistence of SCP and ICP had the fastest cognitive decline (βCHARLS = −0.051 [−0.080, −0.021]; βSHARE = −0.024 [−0.043, −0.004]; pooled β = −0.035 [−0.061, −0.009] z-score/year) and highest risk of dementia (ORCHARLS = 1.77 [1.42, 2.20]; HRSHARE = 2.94 [2.42, 3.59]; pooled relative risk = 2.29 [1.38, 3.77]).

Conclusions

The study suggested that interviewer-reported cognitive problems may be early indicators of cognitive decline and dementia in middle-aged and older adults. A combination of self- and interviewer-reported cognitive problems showed the strongest associations with cognitive decline and dementia.

Peer Review reports

Background

Dementia, primarily characterized by progressive decline in cognitive and functional abilities, is one of the leading causes of disability and mortality in the elderly worldwide [1, 2]. Due to increasing dementia cases and lack of disease-modifying treatments, early identification of individuals at high risk of developing dementia or at the preclinical stage has received considerable attention [3,4,5]. Particularly, modifying risk factors and multidomain lifestyle interventions in the early time may help to delay or prevent cognitive decline and dementia for them [4, 6, 7].

Self-reported cognitive problems (SCP), generally known as subjective cognitive decline (SCD) or subjective cognitive complaints (SCC), have been employed as one of the earliest symptomatic manifestations preceding the onset of dementia across different populations [8, 9]. A recent meta-analysis of longitudinal studies suggested that SCP were associated with a 90% increased risk of incident dementia [10]. Although considered as early indicators of dementia, many adults reported SCP probably due to normal aging and affective symptoms (i.e., depression and anxiety), whilst the majority of individuals with SCP wouldn't show progressive cognitive decline [11, 12]. Therefore, simply using the SCP to identify high-risk persons may additionally include many individuals unlikely to develop moderate-to-severe cognitive impairment. Fortunately, informants can notice mild cognitive dysfunction at a slightly more advanced stage than subjects themselves but still before the occurrence of dementia [11]. Previous evidence supported that informant-reported cognitive problems were also indicative of dementia and that the combined associations of self- and informant-reported cognitive problems were stronger comparing to the independent associations of SCP [9, 13, 14]. However, it is of difficulty to ask respective informants of all respondents, especially for those who live alone, in large-scale community studies, whereas obtaining feedback from interviewers is relatively feasible and costs less. Despite large application potentials, it remains unclear whether interviewer-reported cognitive problems (ICP) are independent indicators of age-related cognitive decline and if so, what are the combined associations of SCP and ICP.

To address the research gaps, we aimed to investigate the independent associations of ICP and combined associations of ICP and SCP with subsequent cognitive decline and dementia in two population-based cohort studies.

Methods

Study population

This study was conducted among participants from 2 prospective cohorts: the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS) and the Survey of Health, Ageing, and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). CHARLS and SHARE were sister cohort studies in the Global Aging Data platform, with similar study designs and objectives. CHARLS was a nationally representative survey of adults 45 years of age or older and their spouses in China [15]. SHARE was a cross-national panel study of people aged 50 and older and their spouses in all 27 countries of the European Union, Switzerland, and Israel [16]. Both cohorts collected sociodemographic, lifestyle, and other health-related information via face-to-face interviews. Detailed descriptions of the cohorts were provided elsewhere [17, 18]. Consistent results from two populations with different cultural and genetic backgrounds could increase the credibility and generalizability of the study findings. CHARLS was ethically approved by the Institutional Review Board at Peking University, and SHARE was ethically approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Mannheim and the Ethics Council of the Max Planck Society. All participants provided signed informed consent.

In the present study, we used wave 1 (2011) for CHARLS and wave 4 (2011) for SHARE as baseline, when exposures of interest were first available simultaneously, and followed up until wave 4 (2018) for CHARLS and wave 8 (2019) for SHARE. Outcomes included cognitive decline and dementia (detailed inclusion and exclusion criteria for each outcome could be found in the Additional file 1: SMethods). Final population for analyzing cognitive decline comprised 10,976 Chinese in CHARLS and 40,499 Europeans in SHARE (Additional file 1: Figure S1). Final population for analyzing dementia consisted of 8112 Chinese and 44,849 Europeans (Additional file 1: Figure S2).

Self- and interviewer-reported cognitive problems

Self-rated memory was obtained from the following survey item: “How would you rate your memory at the present time? Would you say it is excellent, very good, good, fair, or poor?”. According to previous studies conducted among Westerners [19, 20], in SHARE, those who reported “fair” or “poor” were treated as having self-reported cognitive problems. Due to cultural differences, Asians may tend to lean toward the negative side and on average be lower than Westerners in self-rating [21, 22]. Hence, in CHARLS, only those who reported “poor” were treated as having SCP.

Interviewer-rated frequency of asking for clarification was derived from the feedback from interviewers after completing the whole investigation. The corresponding item was as follows: “Did the respondent ask for clarification on any questions? Never, almost never, now and then, often, very often, or always?”. In both cohorts, participants whose interviewers reported “often”, “very often”, or “always” were defined as having interviewer-reported cognitive problems. Based on the combined status of SCP and ICP (SCP × ICP), participants were classified into four groups: Non-SCP & Non-ICP, SCP & Non-ICP, Non-SCP & ICP, and SCP & ICP.

Cognitive function

Cognitive function (including two domains: episodic memory and executive function) was measured biennially, with the same cognitive tests for episodic memory and different cognitive tests for executive function used in two cohorts. In line with previous studies [23,24,25,26], tests for episodic memory comprised immediate and delayed word recall for 10 unrelated words. The episodic memory score was calculated as the mean of two recall tasks in CHARLS (ranging from 0 to 10) and the sum of those in SHARE (0-20). In CHARLS, the executive function score included 3 measurements: time orientation (0–5), numerical ability (0–5), and pentagon drawing test (0–1), with a total range of 0 to 11. Differently, in SHARE, the executive function score was the total number of animals named within 1 min in the verbal fluency test. Due to a small number of outliers in this test, scores greater than 45 were re-coded to 45. Therefore, the global cognitive function score was the sum of immediate word recall, delayed word recall, time orientation, numerical ability, and pentagon drawing test in CHARLS, which varied from 0 to 21, whereas that was the sum of immediate word recall, delayed word recall, and verbal fluency test in SHARE, ranging from 0 to 65. To pool results at the same scale across cohorts, cognitive function scores were individually z-transformed in each cohort, with higher scores indicating better cognitive function.

Dementia

In CHARLS, participants were asked whether they were diagnosed with memory-related diseases that included not only dementia but also brain atrophy and Parkinson’s disease. Therefore, we defined probable dementia with the operational criteria (detailed case definition methods could be found in the Additional file 1: SMethods) used in the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) [27]. Briefly, objective cognitive function tests, informant-reported cognitive status of respondents, functional status, and diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease, which were only all available in the wave 4 of CHARLS [28], were used together to identify those with probable dementia. Namely, in CHARLS, probable dementia cases were only available at the last follow-up phase (wave 4). Differently, in SHARE, the status of diagnosed dementia was reported by respondents themselves or proxy respondents at each follow-up phase [29].

Other covariates

The following baseline characteristics were identified as covariates in our study, mainly involving sociodemographic factors, lifestyles, and health conditions. In CHARLS, sociodemographic factors included age (y), gender (male or female), residence (urban or rural), marital status (married or others), education level (illiterate, ≤ primary school, junior high school, or ≥ high school), and household income (in tertiles); lifestyles comprised smoking status (never, former, or current), drinking status (never, former, or current), sleep duration (≤5.0, 5.1–6.0, 6.1–7.0, 7.1–8.0, or >8.0 h), and physical activity level (metabolic equivalent multipliers weighted, in tertiles) [30]; health conditions consisted of BMI (<18.5, 18.5–23.9, 24–27.9, or ≥28 kg/m2), depressive symptoms (scores of CESD-10 ≥12, yes or no), restriction on ADL (difficulty in performing one or more activities of daily living, yes or no), and physician-diagnosed history of hypertension, diabetes, heart-related diseases, stroke, and cancer (yes or no) [31].

In SHARE, sociodemographic factors included age, gender, residence, marital status, education level (primary school, middle school, high school, or ≥ college), and household income; lifestyles comprised smoking status, drinking status, vigorous physical activity (>1 per week, 1 per week, or <1 per week), and moderate physical activity (>1 per week, 1 per week, or <1 per week); health conditions consisted of BMI (<18.5, 18.5–24.9, 25–29.9, or ≥30 kg/m2), depressive symptoms (scores of EURO-D ≥4, yes or no), restriction on ADL, and physician-diagnosed history of hypertension, diabetes, heart-related diseases, stroke, and cancer [32]; country (e.g., Austria, Germany, Sweden) was also modeled to explain between-country differences [25, 29]. In addition, we included follow-up year and baseline cognitive function score in the multivariate model when analyzing cognitive decline in two cohorts.

Statistical analyses

Baseline characteristics of the study participants grouped by the combined status of SCP and ICP were compared using the chi-squared test (χ2) for categorical variables and one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for continuous variables. Missing values of categorical variables were imputed to a separate category. Age-specific and gender-specific prevalence of SCP and ICP in 2 cohorts was calculated, respectively.

Generalized estimating equation (GEE) models were used to examine the association between the independent and combined status of SCP and ICP and cognitive decline, with β coefficients of the cross-product term of exposure and follow-up year indicating mean differences in rates of cognitive decline across groups. As for dementia, cox proportional hazard regression models were performed to calculate HRs and 95% CIs for relations between the independent and combined status of SCP and ICP and risk of diagnosed dementia in SHARE. Survival time was defined as the time from baseline to the date of incident dementia, loss to follow-up, or study endpoint, whichever came first. Proportional hazard assumption was tested and verified by including a cross-product term with time in the model [33]. In CHARLS, logistic regression models were utilized to estimate ORs of probable dementia and 95% CIs. The final multivariate models adjusted for potential confounders including sociodemographic factors, lifestyles, health conditions (without physician-diagnosed history of chronic conditions), country (only in SHARE), and baseline cognitive function score (only when analyzing cognitive decline). Results from 2 cohorts were then pooled using the inverse variance-weighted random-effects models, which allowed for between-study heterogeneity [34].

We performed an exploratory analysis to investigate whether individuals with both SCP and ICP were at the highest risk of age-related cognitive decline. We conducted several sensitivity analyses to test the robustness of findings: (1) multiple imputation for missing covariates; (2) without excluding those regarded as suspected dementia at baseline; (3) excluding those with the lowest 10 percentages in cognitive function at baseline (only when analyzing cognitive decline); (4) excluding those diagnosed with diseases severely impairing cognition during follow-up (only when analyzing cognitive decline); (5) excluding those with stroke or cancer at baseline; (6) additionally adjusting for physician-diagnosed history of chronic conditions; (7) additionally adjusting for baseline cognitive function (only when analyzing dementia); (8) additionally adjusting for self-rated hearing; (9) excluding those with self-rated poor hearing at baseline. Further, to examine whether the current Asians-specific and Westerners-specific definitions of SCP were appropriate, we exchanged the definitions in 2 cohorts and repeated the analyses. Stratified analyses were performed across subgroups based on major covariates (dichotomized). Effect modification was detected by adding two-way interaction terms of exposure and covariates or three-way interaction terms of exposure, follow-up year, and covariates in the models. All the above statistical analyses were conducted using SAS (version 9.4) and R (version 4.0.5). A two-tailed P-value <0.05 was considered to be statistically significant.

Results

Participant characteristics

In the population for analyzing cognitive decline, among 10,976 Chinese (57.7 ± 8.7 years) in CHARLS, 49.9% were female and 37.2% had education levels higher than primary school; of 40,499 Europeans (64.6 ± 9.4 years) in SHARE, 57.5% were female and 80.6% had education levels higher than primary school (Table 1). Baseline characteristics of the population for analyzing dementia were similar (data not shown). Participants with both SCP and ICP were more likely to be older; have lower education level, household income, and physical activity level; and have depressive symptoms, restriction on ADL, hypertension, heart-related diseases, stroke, and poorer cognitive function at baseline. Besides, proportions of Non-SCP & Non-ICP decreased by age and those of SCP & ICP increased in 2 cohorts (Additional file 1: Figure S3).

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of the study population in CHARLS (N = 10,976) and SHARE (N = 40,499)

SCP and ICP with cognitive decline

During an average of 5.4 and 5.7 years of follow-up, rates of cognitive decline were −0.039 (−0.044, −0.034) z-score/year for Chinese and −0.027 (−0.029, −0.025) z-score/year for Europeans. SCP showed no statistical relationships with global cognitive score decline (pooled β = −0.003 [−0.008, 0.003]; Additional file 1: Table S1) while SCP were related to faster cognitive decline in the domain of episodic memory in Chinese (β = −0.023 [−0.036, −0.010]; data not shown). Chinese with ICP had faster cognitive decline (β = −0.025 [−0.044, −0.006]) while the association was not observed among Europeans (β = −0.006 [−0.017, 0.006]; pooled β = −0.014 [−0.032, 0.005]). When further mutually adjusting for SCP and ICP, results did not materially change. In the joint association analyses, compared to participants without SCP and ICP, those with coexistence of SCP and ICP were associated with faster cognitive decline (βChinese = −0.051 [−0.080, −0.021]; βEuropeans = −0.024 [−0.043, −0.004]; pooled β = −0.035 [−0.061, −0.009] z-score/year) whereas the corresponding differences were non-significant for those with only SCP or ICP (Table 2). Significant relations between coexistence of SCP and ICP with episodic memory-specific and executive function-specific cognitive decline were also found (Additional file 1: Figure S4).

Table 2 Combined association of SCP and ICP with cognitive decline among Chinese (N = 10,976) and European (N = 40,499) middle-aged and older participants

SCP and ICP with dementia

At the 7th year of follow-up in CHARLS, 1120 incident probable dementia cases were identified while a total of 1524 incident diagnosed dementia cases were reported during the follow-up (mean = 6.0 years) in SHARE. In the multivariate models, SCP and ICP were respectively associated with 73% (30%, 129%) and 40% (10%, 79%) increased risk of dementia, with similar results after mutually adjusting for SCP and ICP (Additional file 1: Table S2). Besides, compared to participants without SCP and ICP, Chinese and Europeans with coexistence of SCP and ICP were associated with higher odds (OR = 1.77 [1.42, 2.20]) and hazards (HR = 2.94 [2.42, 3.59]) of dementia respectively (pooled relative risk = 2.29 [1.38, 3.77]; Table 3). Associations for those with only SCP or ICP were attenuated but remained significant, with pooled relative risk ranging from 1.38 to 1.72.

Table 3 Combined association of SCP and ICP with dementia among Chinese (N = 8112) and European (N = 44,849) middle-aged and older participants

Exploratory analysis, subgroup analyses, and sensitivity analyses

In the exploratory analysis, we observed that the SCP & ICP group had the fastest cognitive decline and highest risk of dementia among four groups (Additional file 1: Table S3-S4). In the subgroup analyses, we found no statistical evidence for effect modification by gender, residence, education level, smoking status, depressive symptoms, and history of chronic conditions. However, the combined associations of SCP and ICP with cognitive decline were stronger among those aged ≥60 in CHARLS (P-interaction <0.001) whereas opposite effect modification results for cognitive decline and dementia were observed in SHARE (both P-interaction = 0.001; Figs. 1 and 2). Additionally, relationships of SCP and ICP with cognitive decline were stronger among those with BMI <24 in CHARLS (P-interaction = 0.016). In the sensitivity analyses, results were similar to the main analysis, indicating the robustness of observed associations (Additional file 1: Table S5-S8). When we exchanged the definitions of SCP in 2 cohorts, the corresponding associations were moderately attenuated.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Subgroup analyses for the combined association of SCP and ICP with cognitive decline in CHARLS (A) and SHARE (B) by comparing SCP & ICP group and Non-SCP & Non-ICP group in the fully adjusted model

Fig. 2
figure 2

Subgroup analyses for the combined association of SCP and ICP with dementia in CHARLS (A) and SHARE (B) by comparing SCP & ICP group and Non-SCP & Non-ICP group in the fully adjusted model

Discussion

In the two community-based prospective cohort studies of Chinese (7-year follow-up) and European (8-year follow-up) middle-aged and older adults without dementia, we observed independent associations of interviewer-reported cognitive problems with faster cognitive decline and higher risk of incident dementia. Additionally, the strongest associations with age-related cognitive decline were found among participants with the co-existence of self- and interviewer-reported cognitive problems.

In the current study, we observed that SCP were related to faster cognitive decline in the domain of episodic memory in CHARLS. Consistent with our results, one community-based study including 4015 older adults (age = 77.7 ± 7.2 years) found that those with memory complaints had faster decline in the domain of episodic memory (β = −0.037 [−0.047, −0.027]) during a mean follow-up of 6 years [35]. Besides, SCP were related to 73% increased risk of developing dementia in our study, which was similar to the pooled results from a meta-analysis (HR = 1.90 [1.52, 2.36]) [10]. Therefore, our findings provided additional evidence to support SCP as indicators of cognitive decline and dementia. In addition, we found that ICP were associated with faster cognitive decline and elevated risk of dementia. Although there is no research exploring whether ICP were related to age-related cognitive decline, several previous studies investigated the associations of informant-reported cognitive problems. For example, one study with 1048 individuals (age = 73.3 ± 7.0 years) from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative showed that worse informant-ECog (Everyday cognition scale) scores were associated with greater decline in multiple neuropsychological tests and increased risk of conversion from mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to dementia [36]. With the large sample size and diverse education levels, these two prospective cohort studies provided strong evidence to support the indicative role of cognitive problems reported by interviewers in age-related cognitive decline.

In the joint association analysis, the coexistence of SCP and ICP was associated with the fastest cognitive decline and highest risk of incident dementia. Similarly, previous studies found that participants with both self- and informant-reported cognitive problems were related to increased risk of cognitive decline and progression from MCI to dementia, with effect estimates greater than those only having one of the two sources of reported cognitive problems [13, 37]. Particularly, one study conducted among community-dwelling older adults (age = 78.7 ± 4.8 years) found that self-reported memory decline may indicate faster cognitive decline in the domain of language while informant-reported memory decline may be indicative of decline in the domain of executive function and memory [38]. Hence, cognitive problems reported by the respondent himself or a third person may represent early signs of future decline in different cognitive domains, which was probably one of the reasons why the coexistence of SCP and ICP showed the strongest relationships with age-related cognitive decline. In addition, compared to informants, obtaining feedback from interviewers may be more feasible and cost less in the community-based studies. Therefore, a combination of self- and interviewer-reported cognitive problems could be employed as a complementary approach to identify those at high risk of age-related cognitive decline, especially in situations when finding appropriate informants was unavailable or difficult.

The present study has a number of strengths. The prospective study design, long-term follow-up, large sample size, inclusion of two populations with different cultural backgrounds and education levels, and careful control of various potential confounders minimized selection bias and reverse causation, thus providing relatively valid estimates of associations. Nevertheless, several limitations should be considered when interpreting the results of our study. The primary limitation is the nature of an observational study design where the observed associations may be impacted by residual and unmeasured confounders. However, we statistically adjusted for a wide range of key risk factors including baseline cognitive function, suggesting that confounding is not a likely explanation for the current findings. Second, the incidence of dementia was measured in an unstandardized way between the two cohorts. However, ways of utilizing the operational criteria and self- or proxy-reported diagnosis to define dementia were both widely used and validated in epidemiological studies [27, 39,40,41]. More studies with standardized ways to define dementia are warranted to confirm and elucidate the observed findings. Another limitation is the possible reverse causality. We excluded dementia cases occurring within the first 2 years of follow-up when analyzing dementia. Additionally, we repeated the analyses for cognitive decline after excluding those diagnosed with diseases severely impairing cognition during the follow-up and observed similar findings. Fourthly, the use of self-rated current memory performance instead of memory change to define SCP may be subject to underestimation. Nevertheless, such situations potentially included more at-risk persons in the reference group and biased our association estimates toward the null. Finally, some participants may be regarded as having ICP due to language barriers or hearing impairment instead of cognitive problems. However, native speakers were recruited and trained to become interviewers, and feedback from interviewers used to define ICP focused on asking for clarification rather than repetition of questions. We also observed similar results after additionally adjusting for self-rated hearing or excluding those with self-rated poor hearing. More studies with diverse methods to define SCP and ICP are warranted to confirm and elucidate the observed findings.

Conclusions

Interviewer-reported cognitive problems and the coexistence of self- and interviewer-reported cognitive problems were associated with faster cognitive decline and higher risk of dementia. The study findings suggested that interviewer-reported cognitive problems may be early indicators of age-related cognitive decline in middle-aged and older adults across different populations. A combination of self- and interviewer-reported cognitive problems could be utilized to identify individuals at high risk of developing cognitive decline and dementia, providing an important time window to delay and prevent dementia.

Availability of data and materials

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the website of the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS) at http://charls.pku.edu.cn/ and website of the Survey of Health, Ageing, and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) at https://www.share-eric.eu/.

Abbreviations

ADL:

Activities of daily living

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

BMI:

Body mass index

CHARLS:

China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study

ECog:

Everyday cognition scale

ELSA:

English Longitudinal Study of Ageing

GEE:

Generalized estimating equation

HR:

Hazard ratio

ICP:

Interviewer-reported cognitive problems

MCI:

Mild cognitive impairment

OR:

Odds ratio

SCC:

Subjective cognitive complaints

SCD:

Subjective cognitive decline

SCP:

Self-reported cognitive problems

SHARE:

Survey of Health, Ageing, and Retirement in Europe

References

  1. GBD 2016 Neurology Collaborators. Global, regional, and national burden of neurological disorders, 1990-2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18(5):459–80.

  2. Weimer DL, Sager MA. Early identification and treatment of Alzheimer’s disease: social and fiscal outcomes. Alzheimers Dement. 2009;5(3):215–26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Tolar M, Abushakra S, Sabbagh M. The path forward in Alzheimer’s disease therapeutics: reevaluating the amyloid cascade hypothesis. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16(11):1553–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;396(10248):413–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Dubois B, Hampel H, Feldman HH, Scheltens P, Aisen P, Andrieu S, et al. Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: definition, natural history, and diagnostic criteria. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12(3):292–323.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, Levälahti E, Ahtiluoto S, Antikainen R, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2015;385(9984):2255–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Aisen PS, Jimenez-Maggiora GA, Rafii MS, Walter S, Raman R. Early-stage Alzheimer disease: getting trial-ready. Nat Rev Neurol. 2022;18(7):389–99.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Jessen F, Amariglio RE, van Boxtel M, Breteler M, Ceccaldi M, Chételat G, et al. A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(6):844–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Rabin LA, Smart CM, Amariglio RE. Subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2017;13:369–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Pike KE, Cavuoto MG, Li L, Wright BJ, Kinsella GJ. Subjective cognitive decline: level of risk for future dementia and mild cognitive impairment, a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Neuropsychol Rev. 2021;32(4):703–35.

  11. Jessen F, Amariglio RE, Buckley RF, van der Flier WM, Han Y, Molinuevo JL, et al. The characterisation of subjective cognitive decline. Lancet Neurol. 2020;19(3):271–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Hill NL, Mogle J, Wion R, Munoz E, DePasquale N, Yevchak AM, et al. Subjective cognitive impairment and affective symptoms: a systematic review. Gerontologist. 2016;56(6):e109–27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Gifford KA, Liu D, Lu Z, Tripodis Y, Cantwell NG, Palmisano J, et al. The source of cognitive complaints predicts diagnostic conversion differentially among nondemented older adults. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(3):319–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Amariglio RE, Donohue MC, Marshall GA, Rentz DM, Salmon DP, Ferris SH, et al. Tracking early decline in cognitive function in older individuals at risk for Alzheimer disease dementia: the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study Cognitive Function Instrument. JAMA Neurol. 2015;72(4):446–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS). http://charls.pku.edu.cn/. Accessed 18 Oct 2023.

  16. Survey of Health, Ageing, and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). https://share-eric.eu/. Accessed 18 Oct 2023.

  17. Zhao Y, Hu Y, Smith JP, Strauss J, Yang G. Cohort profile: the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS). Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(1):61–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Börsch-Supan A, Brandt M, Hunkler C, Kneip T, Korbmacher J, Malter F, et al. Data Resource Profile: the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(4):992–1001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Gardner RC, Langa KM, Yaffe K. Subjective and objective cognitive function among older adults with a history of traumatic brain injury: a population-based cohort study. PLoS Med. 2017;14(3): e1002246.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Franco Y, Saenz J, Jang Y, Ho J. Differences in self-rated memory by race/ethnicity. Innov Aging. 2021;5(Supplement_1):548.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. French DJ, Browning C, Kendig H, Luszcz MA, Saito Y, Sargent-Cox K, et al. A simple measure with complex determinants: investigation of the correlates of self-rated health in older men and women from three continents. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:649.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Ha Choi J, Miyamoto Y. Cultural differences in self-rated health: the role of influence and adjustment. Jpn Psychol Res. 2022;64(2):156–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Rong H, Lai X, Jing R, Wang X, Fang H, Mahmoudi E. Association of sensory impairments with cognitive decline and depression among older adults in China. JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(9):e2014186.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Huang Y, Zhang S, Shen J, Yang J, Chen X, Li W, et al. Association of plasma uric acid levels with cognitive function among non-hyperuricemia adults: a prospective study. Clin Nutr. 2022;41(3):645–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Cohn-Schwartz E, Levinsky M, Litwin H. Social network type and subsequent cognitive health among older Europeans. Int Psychogeriatr. 2021;33(5):495–504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Schwartz E, Litwin H. Warfare exposure in later life and cognitive function: the moderating role of social connectedness. Psychiatry Res. 2019;278:258–62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Ahmadi-Abhari S, Guzman-Castillo M, Bandosz P, Shipley MJ, Muniz-Terrera G, Singh-Manoux A, et al. Temporal trend in dementia incidence since 2002 and projections for prevalence in England and Wales to 2040: modelling study. BMJ. 2017;358:j2856.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Meng Q, Wang H, Strauss J, Langa KM, Chen X, Wang M, et al. Validation of neuropsychological tests for the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study Harmonized Cognitive Assessment Protocol. Int Psychogeriatr. 2019;31(12):1709–19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Wu JJ, Wang HX, Yao W, Yan Z, Pei JJ. Late-life depression and the risk of dementia in 14 countries: a 10-year follow-up study from the Survey of Health, ageing and retirement in Europe. J Affect Disord. 2020;274:671–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Deng Y, Paul DR. The relationships between depressive symptoms, functional health status, physical activity, and the availability of recreational facilities: a rural-urban comparison in middle-aged and older Chinese adults. Int J Behav Med. 2018;25(3):322–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Cheng ST, Chan AC. The Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale in older Chinese: thresholds for long and short forms. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;20(5):465–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Chen S, Ford TJ, Jones PB, Cardinal RN. Prevalence, progress, and subgroup disparities in pharmacological antidepressant treatment of those who screen positive for depressive symptoms: a repetitive cross-sectional study in 19 European countries. Lancet Reg Health Eur. 2022;17:100368.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Ma Y, Wolters FJ, Chibnik LB, Licher S, Ikram MA, Hofman A, et al. Variation in blood pressure and long-term risk of dementia: a population-based cohort study. PLoS Med. 2019;16(11):e1002933.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Pan A, Sun Q, Bernstein AM, Schulze MB, Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, et al. Red meat consumption and mortality: results from 2 prospective cohort studies. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(7):555–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Arvanitakis Z, Leurgans SE, Fleischman DA, Schneider JA, Rajan KB, Pruzin JJ, et al. Memory complaints, dementia, and neuropathology in older blacks and whites. Ann Neurol. 2018;83(4):718–29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Nosheny RL, Jin C, Neuhaus J, Insel PS, Mackin RS, Weiner MW. Study partner-reported decline identifies cognitive decline and dementia risk. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. 2019;6(12):2448–59.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Qi XM, Gu L, Tang HD, Chen SD, Ma JF. Association of source of memory complaints and increased risk of cognitive impairment and cognitive decline: a community-based study. Chin Med J (Engl). 2018;131(8):894–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Numbers K, Crawford JD, Kochan NA, Draper B, Sachdev PS, Brodaty H. Participant and informant memory-specific cognitive complaints predict future decline and incident dementia: findings from the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study. PLoS One. 2020;15(5):e0232961.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Cadar D, Lassale C, Davies H, Llewellyn DJ, Batty GD, Steptoe A. Individual and area-based socioeconomic factors associated with dementia incidence in England: evidence from a 12-year follow-up in the English longitudinal study of ageing. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(7):723–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Meijer E, Casanova M, Kim H, Llena-Nozal A, Lee J. Economic costs of dementia in 11 countries in Europe: Estimates from nationally representative cohorts of a panel study. Lancet Reg Health Europe. 2022;20:100445.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Veronese N, Koyanagi A, Dominguez LJ, Maggi S, Soysal P, Bolzetta F, et al. Multimorbidity increases the risk of dementia: a 15 year follow-up of the SHARE study. Age and Ageing. 2023;52(4):afad052.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to express genuine gratitude to the participants and staff of the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study and Survey of Health, Ageing, and Retirement in Europe, who contributed greatly to the academic community and made this study possible.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Key R&D Program of China (No: 2022YFC2010106). The funder of the study had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, writing of the report or involved in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

C.Y., X.X., and Y.H. designed the study; Y.H. and H.C. performed the statistical analyses; C.Y., X.X., Y.H., and H.C. interpreted the data; C.Y., Y.H., and H.C. had full access and verified all the data in the study; Y.H. drafted the manuscript; C.Y., X.X., H.C., M.G., X.L., T.P., and S.R. further revised the manuscript; C.Y. and X.X. supervised the data analysis and interpretation; C.Y. had the primary responsibility for the study final content. All authors critically reviewed the manuscript and approved the final draft.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Xin Xu or Changzheng Yuan.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

CHARLS was ethically approved by the Institutional Review Board at Peking University (00001052-11014, 00001052-11015). SHARE is subject to continuous ethics review, and was ethically approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Mannheim and the Ethics Council of the Max Planck Society. In addition, the country implementations of SHARE were reviewed and approved by the respective ethics committees or institutional review boards whenever this was required. The current study utilizing data from CHARLS and SHARE was ethically approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the School of Public Health of Zhejiang University (ZGL202112-4). All participants provided signed informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:

 SMethods, Figures S1-S4, and Tables S1-S8. SMethods. [Supplementary Methods]. Figure S1. [Flow chart of participants selection for analyzing cognitive decline]. Figure S2. [Flow chart of participants selection for analyzing dementia]. Figure S3. [Age-specific and gender-specific prevalence of self-reported cognitive problems (SCP) and interviewer-reported cognitive problems (ICP) in CHARLS (A) and SHARE (B)]. Table S1. [Association of SCP and ICP on cognitive decline among Chinese (N = 10,976) and European (N = 40,499) middle-aged and older participants]. Figure S4. [Combined association of SCP and ICP on cognitive decline in the domains (episodic memory and executive function) among Chinese (N = 10,976) and European (N = 40,499) middle-aged and older participants]. Table S2. [Association of SCP and ICP on dementia among Chinese (N = 8112) and European (N = 44,849) middle-aged and older participants]. Table S3. [Combined association of SCP and ICP on cognitive decline among Chinese (N = 10,976) and European (N = 40,499) middle-aged and older participants (SCP & ICP group as reference)]. Table S4. [Combined association of SCP and ICP on dementia among Chinese (N = 8112) and European (N = 44,849) middle-aged and older participants (SCP & ICP group as reference)]. Table S5. [Sensitivity analyses for the combined association of SCP and ICP on cognitive decline in CHARLS]. Table S6. [Sensitivity analyses for the combined association of SCP and ICP on cognitive decline in SHARE]. Table S7. [Sensitivity analyses for the combined association of SCP and ICP on probable dementia in CHARLS]. Table S8. [Sensitivity analyses for the combined association of SCP and ICP on diagnosed dementia in SHARE].

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Huang, Y., Chen, H., Gao, M. et al. Self- and interviewer-reported cognitive problems in relation to cognitive decline and dementia: results from two prospective studies. BMC Med 22, 23 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-023-03147-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-023-03147-4

Keywords