Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Real-world nudging, pricing, and mobile physical activity coaching was insufficient to improve lifestyle behaviours and cardiometabolic health: the Supreme Nudge parallel cluster-randomised controlled supermarket trial

A Commentary to this article was published on 27 February 2024

Abstract

Background

Context-specific interventions may contribute to sustained behaviour change and improved health outcomes. We evaluated the real-world effects of supermarket nudging and pricing strategies and mobile physical activity coaching on diet quality, food-purchasing behaviour, walking behaviour, and cardiometabolic risk markers.

Methods

This parallel cluster-randomised controlled trial included supermarkets in socially disadvantaged neighbourhoods across the Netherlands with regular shoppers aged 30–80 years. Supermarkets were randomised to receive co-created nudging and pricing strategies promoting healthier purchasing (N = 6) or not (N = 6). Nudges targeted 9% of supermarket products and pricing strategies 3%. Subsequently, participants were individually randomised to a control (step counter app) or intervention arm (step counter and mobile coaching app) to promote walking. The primary outcome was the average change in diet quality (low (0) to high (150)) over all follow-up time points measured with a validated 40-item food frequency questionnaire at baseline and 3, 6, and 12 months. Secondary outcomes included healthier food purchasing (loyalty card-derived), daily step count (step counter app), cardiometabolic risk markers (lipid profile and HbA1c via finger prick, and waist circumference via measuring tape), and supermarket customer satisfaction (questionnaire-based: very unsatisfied (1) to very satisfied (7)), evaluated using linear mixed-models. Healthy supermarket sales (an exploratory outcome) were analysed via controlled interrupted time series analyses.

Results

Of 361 participants (162 intervention, 199 control), 73% were female, the average age was 58 (SD 11) years, and 42% were highly educated. Compared to the control arm, the intervention arm showed no statistically significant average changes over time in diet quality (β − 1.1 (95% CI − 3.8 to 1.7)), percentage healthy purchasing (β 0.7 ( − 2.7 to 4.0)), step count (β − 124.0 (− 723.1 to 475.1), or any of the cardiometabolic risk markers. Participants in the intervention arm scored 0.3 points (0.1 to 0.5) higher on customer satisfaction on average over time. Supermarket-level sales were unaffected (β − 0.0 (− 0.0 to 0.0)).

Conclusions

Co-created nudging and pricing strategies that predominantly targeted healthy products via nudges were unable to increase healthier food purchases and intake nor improve cardiometabolic health. The mobile coaching intervention did not affect step count. Governmental policy measures are needed to ensure more impactful supermarket modifications that promote healthier purchases.

Trial registration

Dutch Trial Register ID NL7064, 30 May 2018, https://www.onderzoekmetmensen.nl/en/trial/20990

Peer Review reports

Background

Cardiometabolic diseases such as cardiovascular diseases and type 2 diabetes are leading causes of morbidity and mortality worldwide [1, 2]. Unhealthy diets and physical inactivity are key modifiable risk factors, which are strongly socioeconomically patterned [3,4,5]. Individual-level interventions to improve lifestyle behaviours such as behavioural counselling are generally not sustainable beyond their duration [6, 7]. Such interventions rely heavily on individual resources like knowledge and motivation and inadequately reach vulnerable populations including those with a low socioeconomic position (SEP) [8,9,10]. Interventions that modify the environmental context surrounding lifestyle choices can enhance healthier lifestyle behaviours for all individuals exposed to this context and reduce cardiometabolic risk on a population level [11].

The supermarket is an important context to modulate food choices and is where about 80% of foods are purchased in the Netherlands [12]. Yet, only 21% of products sold in Dutch supermarkets are recommended by the Dutch dietary guidelines [13]. Nudging and pricing strategies may help customers choose more healthy products. Nudging—environmental changes that promote a certain choice, without removing the alternative choice—has been shown to increase healthier purchasing or sales, mainly in highly controlled experimental contexts [14], while mixed effects are observed in real-world contexts [15,16,17,18]. Pricing strategies such as food taxes and subsidies are effective in stimulating healthier choices [19, 20] and are a recommended policy of the World Health Organization [21]. However, impactful food pricing policies are politically difficult to implement and therefore potentially not sufficient to achieve a large-scale impact on health. Implementing various types of nudging and pricing strategies simultaneously across food groups are probably needed to substantially improve dietary patterns [18, 22].

Wearable devices or smartphones can provide context-specific cues to stimulate physical activity by prompting at decision-making moments [23, 24], rendering them context-specific, yet not limited to a physical setting or place. The promotion of walking is a relevant target for preventing cardiometabolic diseases for individuals across SEP and age groups; it is free of cost to users and easily incorporated into daily activities. Existing mobile coaching strategies generally focus on the motivation to change physical activity and rarely consider the individual context [25,26,27,28]. Providing context-specific coaching messages may enhance the context action relationship and promote walking habits [29].

Most previous real-world studies evaluated effects of nudging, pricing, and mobile coaching strategies separately, with mixed or small effects [15, 18, 20, 30,31,32]. Only two real-world studies evaluated effects of a combined nudging and pricing intervention, but these targeted only a single or combination of specific food groups and evaluated short-term effects (≤ 6 months) on supermarket-level sales [33, 34]. Information on long-term and real-world effects of nudging, pricing, and mobile coaching strategies on various lifestyle behaviours and health outcomes would provide a strong methodological underpinning on their effectiveness across populations [35].

Within the Supreme Nudge project [36], we evaluated the real-world effects of nudging and pricing strategies promoting healthy products and a mobile coaching app promoting walking on individual-level diet quality, purchasing behaviour, walking behaviour, cardiometabolic risk markers, customer satisfaction, and various psychosocial factors among Dutch adults after 6 to 12 months compared to no interventions. We hypothesised that nudging, pricing, and mobile physical activity coaching would improve healthier food purchasing and intake, daily step count, customer satisfaction, and cardiometabolic risk markers. Supermarket-level sales data were used to assess whether selective trial participation influenced our results, by exploring changes in supermarket sales trends in intervention supermarkets compared to control supermarkets.

Methods

Trial design

We conducted a parallel cluster-randomised controlled trial (RCT)—the Supreme Nudge trial—in 12 Dutch supermarkets from one chain in socially disadvantaged neighbourhoods. Supermarkets were randomised to a control arm (n = 6) or intervention arm receiving nudging and pricing strategies to promote healthy food and beverage purchases (n = 6). Participants were additionally randomised to a mobile physical activity coaching app intervention (‘SNapp’), receiving either a mobile coaching app and a step counter app (intervention arm) or only the step counter app (control arm). The duration of intervention implementation was six (n = 4 supermarket clusters) to 12 months (n = 8) depending on supermarket enrolment date.

More detailed information on the trial design is published in a protocol paper [37], including a correction paper detailing protocol changes due to the COVID-19 pandemic [38]. We also briefly summarise these protocol changes further down. The current paper focusses mainly on the supermarket-related outcomes. In a separate paper, we will describe and evaluate the mobile physical activity coaching app intervention in more detail. This trial was prospectively registered in the Dutch Trial Register (ID NL7064) on 30 May 2018. The reporting this paper presents is in accordance with the 2010 Consort statement for cluster-randomised trials [39].

Supermarkets

Eligible supermarkets were regular stores (i.e. no convenience store) located in socially disadvantaged neighbourhoods (below average postal code SEP-scores according to The Netherlands Institute for Social Research) [40] and had implemented an adapted cash-register system to allow for pricing strategies. Of 69 eligible supermarkets, we selected 12 balancing the lowest number of competing supermarkets within a 2-km radius and the highest number of potentially eligible residents living in the neighbourhood. There were no systematic differences between supermarkets at baseline.

Participants

Eligible individuals were aged 30–80 years, regular shoppers at a participating supermarket (self-reported to purchase > 50% weekly groceries at a participating supermarket), planning on continuing shopping there for the study period, and could communicate in Dutch. Participants had to provide written informed constant prior to study enrolment. For additional individual-level randomisation to the mobile coaching app, participants had to use a smartphone with a mobile data plan and at least Android 8 or iOS 13, have experience with mobile text messaging, and be able to climb a flight of stairs.

Recruitment

We used a mix of passive and active recruitment strategies prior to the intervention implementation, as described elsewhere [41]. Passive strategies entailed, for example, online news articles in local newspapers, flyers and invitation letters sent to every household in the supermarkets’ neighbourhood, flyers in supermarkets, and e-mail invitations to supermarket customer panels. Active recruitment strategies included word-of-mouth promotion among included participants and in-store recruitment by research staff.

Intervention

Nudging and pricing strategies

Supermarket interventions consisted of a combination of nudging and pricing strategies to promote healthier food purchasing. We targeted a wide range of healthy product groups (i.e. vegetables, fruits, whole-grain products, fish, low-fat milk and yogurt, low-fat cheese, legumes, unsaturated fats, unsalted nuts, sugar-free bottled beverages, and tea) to promote a healthy dietary pattern which is in accordance with the Dutch dietary guidelines [42]. Strategies were developed in a co-creative process with supermarket stakeholders and interventionists [37, 43]. Supermarket interventions were implemented by supermarket staff so that they would be sustainable and scalable beyond the duration of the project. We used data of the Netherlands Nutrition Centre to classify products recommended (‘healthy’) or non-recommended (‘unhealthy’) [44]. Each of the included supermarkets offered around 12,500 foods and beverages, of which 19% were classified as healthy (Additional file 1: Table S1).

We used the Typology of Interventions in Proximal Physical Micro-Environments (TIPPME) to classify nudges into placement nudges (availability and the positioning of products) and property nudges (functionality or design of products, or highlighting specific product information) [45]. Our placement nudges adjusted the shelf location and increased the number of healthy products prominently placed. The property nudges consisted of information symbols highlighting healthy product’s tastiness, convenience, or popularity, applied as shelf feedback strips, shelf-stoppers, healthy suggestions shelf-banners, and shelf-labels. The self-labels targeted all available healthy products within a food group (Table 1). Nudges were implemented for the entire trial period on approximately 1100 healthy products simultaneously (i.e. 9% of supermarket assortment). Photographic examples can be found in Additional file 2: Figures S1a–k.

Table 1 Overview of implemented supermarket interventions in the Supreme Nudge trial

Pricing strategies consisted of salient price reductions of healthy products and non-salient price increases of unhealthy products, wherever possible, within the same food group (Table 1). Price reductions were by 25% in food groups with only healthy options (e.g. fresh vegetables) or by 10% on healthy products when combined with a 15% increase on unhealthy products in the same food group. The supermarket chain allowed a maximum of 200 price changes per week and products in regular weekly price promotions were excluded from the pricing strategies. Pricing strategies targeted 3% of the total assortment and were implemented for ~ 3 consecutive weeks in one or multiple food groups, after which they switched to other food groups (Additional file 1: Table S2).

Mobile physical activity coaching app

The mobile physical activity coaching app consisted of a smartphone step counter app and dynamically tailored coaching content [46]. The app continuously quantified step count through the smartphone’s built-in pedometer or accelerometer. GPS data comparing participants’ location to a database of pre-selected locations suitable for walking every 2 min served as input for the coaching content. Three types of content were delivered via push notifications in a Telegram Messenger chat account: (1) feedback tailored to participants’ step count levels, (2) contextually tailored prompts when participants were near walking locations, and (3) advice tailored to behavioural change technique preferences measured at baseline. Coaching content was adapted to be readily understood by individuals with a low SEP (language level ‘Dutch B1’). A server-based Python program that interacted with databases containing user data and coaching messages automatically selected appropriate messages to send to participants using a rule-based system. Participants received at least two and maximally six messages daily.

Study outcomes and data collection

Measurements took place at baseline (T0), after 3 months (T1), after 6 months (T2), and after 12 months (T3). The primary outcome was the change in total diet quality (Dutch Healthy Diet 2015 index) from baseline until 12 months [47], measured with a validated short 40-item food frequency questionnaire (FFQ). The short FFQ asked about the average dietary intake during the past month from 15 components representing the food-based Dutch dietary guidelines of 2015. For each component, a score between 0 and 10 was assigned, resulting in a total score ranging from 0 (low diet quality) to 150 (high diet quality) [48, 49]. Secondary outcomes were as follows: the total percentage of healthier food purchasing (calculated based on grams purchased derived from weekly data collected via loyalty cards), daily step counts (step counter app) [46], cardiometabolic risk markers self-assessed by participants at home (glycated haemoglobin (HbA1c), low-density lipoprotein (LDL) cholesterol, high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol, total cholesterol (TC), TC/HDL-ratio, triglycerides (TG) collected via a finger prick measurement, and waist circumference via a measuring tape, presented and analysed stratified by sex) [37], and questionnaire items relating to overall customer satisfaction, food-decision styles in relation to vegetables and to snacks (reflective, habitual, and impulsive) [50,51,52,53,54], social cognitive factors related to nudges (heath goals, healthy shopping, perceived social norm, and attractiveness of healthy foods) [55,56,57,58,59,60], and acceptance and awareness of nudges after six or 12 months. The purchase data were collected weekly over the complete duration of the intervention and recalculated to an average purchase per blocks of 4 weeks, leading to 13 time points. Step count data was collected daily (Additional file 1: Table S3 [37, 38]). As explorative outcomes, we described medication use, diet quality scores of various food groups, and percentage of healthy purchasing within various food groups per time point. Further details on data collection methods are described elsewhere [37, 41].

At the supermarket-level, we explored whether nudging and pricing strategies changed supermarket-level sales trends to account for potential selective trial participation [61]. We used sales data of the intervention period plus 6 months of pre-intervention data. Sales data comprised the number of products and Euros spent on healthy and unhealthy products per supermarket per week, based on 63 product groups which were pre-defined by the supermarket chain. We primarily investigated changes in the total percentage of healthy product sales of all sales in intervention supermarkets compared to control supermarkets during the intervention period, while taking into account the pre-intervention sales trends. We assigned the 63 product groups into 10 overarching food groups and examined the percentage change in sales from: fruits, vegetables, legumes, and nuts, grain products, milk and yogurt products, cheese, meat products, meat substitutes and eggs, fish, oils, fats and herbs and spices, non-alcoholic beverages, products from all remaining food products (e.g. pre-packaged meals, and baking products), and sweet and savoury snacks of total sales. Lastly, we examined changes in total sales revenue (Euros).

We monitored the implementation fidelity of the nudging and pricing strategies monthly to bi-monthly as the trial progressed. Research staff visited intervention supermarkets and quantitatively scored intervention components separately by their implementation status (none/minimal implementation scored 1, to (almost) optimal implementation scored 5). A summary score of all components was calculated for each supermarket by time point.

Sample size

The trial required 141 participants in each supermarket arm to detect an average change of 5 points in the diet quality score by the end of the intervention period, assuming a 14.1 point standard deviation (SD) of the average change (power = 80%; two-sided alpha = 0.05). The expected 5-point change was based on results from a virtual supermarket experiment which indicated a 4% increase in healthier purchasing following a combination of nudging and salient pricing strategies [22]. We did not use a design factor since baseline data of diet quality among participants from the first eight enrolled supermarkets showed no correlation for observations between study sites (intra-cluster correlation coefficient (ICC) = 0.00) [37]. To allow for 25% drop out, 176 participants needed to be recruited per arm, resulting in 352 participants.

Randomisation and blinding

Twelve supermarkets were block-randomised by the first author to the control arm or the intervention arm, using a web-based random-number generator tool with blocks of four supermarkets. Individual-level randomisation in the mobile physical activity coaching app intervention or only the step counter app was conducted via a cloud-based data-management software application (Castor Electronic Data Capture). We used variable block randomisation alternating block sizes of two, four, and six, with equal weight for both groups. A research assistant conducted the randomisation of participants eligible for the coaching app intervention.

Blinding of participants was not feasible due to the nature of the interventions. Participants were not actively informed about the content of the supermarket interventions and not about the intervention allocation of their supermarket location, not prior to their study enrolment nor during the trial. Those randomised in the coaching app intervention were actively informed since they were requested to install the app(s).

Brief summary of protocol changes

Due to the COVID-19 pandemic and its associated governmental restrictions (e.g. social distancing measures), the main protocol underwent some notable changes [38]. As we were unable to perform physical measurements face-to-face, we abandoned planned blood pressure measurements. Instead, waist circumference and blood markers including HbA1c and lipid profile were measured remotely via a self-assessed finger prick. Furthermore, the lack of active recruitment opportunities required a revision of recruitment goals. Due to a lower expected response rate, we recruited from twelve rather than eight supermarket locations. Due to the fixed project end-date, the additional four stores could only be followed-up for six instead of 12 months. The expected lower response rates also forced us to combine intervention arms, leading us to comparing a combined nudging and pricing arm to a control arm rather than also investigating the independent effect of nudging strategies. Finally, in response to the expected lower response rates and participant burden of self-assessment of cardiometabolic risk markers, we revised our primary outcome to only include changes in diet quality, as was already part of the primary outcome, and to exclude systolic blood pressure, LDL cholesterol, and HbA1c from the primary outcome. These changes required us to recruit 352 participants instead of the originally planned 1485.

Statistical analyses

Population characteristics described were stratified by trial arm to visually inspect potential baseline differences as mean (SD) for normally distributed continuous variables, median (IQR) for skewed continuous variables, or frequencies (percentage) for categorical variables.

For evaluation of the primary outcome, we used a linear mixed model with group allocation as independent variable and the diet quality score at T1, T2, and T3 as dependent variable. The model included the covariates diet quality at T0, to account for regression to the mean, and time as categorical variable, considering the varying number of missing values per time point. Interaction between time and treatment indicated no time-specific intervention effects so this interaction was left out of the model. All models included random intercepts at the participant-level and the supermarket-level. The addition of random slopes did not improve the model fit and were therefore not included. Residuals were confirmed to be normally distributed via residual plots. Missing data were not imputed since mixed models use the maximum available data, even if participants had incomplete data. We reported the average within- and between group change in diet quality over all follow-up time points with a 95% confidence interval (CI) for the intervention arm, compared to the control arm and compared to the baseline measurement. We also reported the effect sizes on total diet quality at T1 and T2.

Changes in all secondary outcomes with the exception of step counts were analysed as described above for the primary outcome. Analysis of step count data followed a similar approach, but with only a random intercept at the participant-level, since their appeared no clustering of step count data between study sites, and including adjustment for smartphone operating system and sensor type used for step count tracking. Variables related to acceptance of nudges were only measured at T3 and therefore presented descriptively per arm by their mean (SD) or frequency (%).

Participants were analysed according to their initial treatment assignment (intention-to-treat), and we included participants who had a baseline measurement and at least one follow-up measurement of diet quality; this was considered our analytical sample. For secondary outcomes, we used an analytical sample per outcome, including those participants with at least a baseline measurement and one follow-up measurement for that outcome, leading to varying sample sizes by outcome.

We conducted several sensitivity analyses. To assess the influence of changes in medication on the cardiometabolic risk markers, we explored medication use over time between the trial arms with descriptive statistics. Descriptive statistics were presented for diet quality by food group and the percentage of healthier food purchasing by food group over time, to provide insights for each time point. We assessed effect modification of nudging and pricing strategies on diet quality by sex, age, and educational level by adding an interaction term between the trial arm and the potential effect modifier to the primary outcome model. When the interaction term was statistically significant (p < 0.10), these effect modifiers were used to explore interaction effects on relevant secondary outcomes. Significant interaction effects were plotted to visualise their direction.

For each explorative supermarket-level outcome, we described the average (mean (SD)) pre-intervention and post-intervention sales for both the intervention and the control supermarkets. We conducted controlled interrupted time series analyses based on a linear mixed model [62, 63]. For each outcome, we tested for changes in the average sales trends over time in intervention supermarkets compared to the control supermarkets, treating the pre-intervention period sales trends as fixed effects and including a random intercept for supermarkets. All analyses excluded the first week of the intervention implementation to allow time for the intervention to be implemented. We modelled changes in sales outcomes as an immediate step change, which assumed an immediate and stable effect of the intervention. Moreover, the average implementation fidelity scores per supermarket per time point were graphically presented, and the median score (range) over time is presented for all intervention stores combined. As a sensitivity analysis, the intervention supermarket with the lowest implementation fidelity score over time was excluded from the analyses of the total percentage of healthy product sales of all sales.

We did not account for multiple testing since our primary, secondary, and explorative analyses all reflect different outcome constructs and were planned in our study protocol. We used a two-sided alpha of 0.05 to define statistical significance, with the exception of the interaction tests for which we used an alpha of 0.10. Analyses were conducted using R statistical software (version 4.2.1), except for the step count data (SPSS version 27).

Results

Individual-level results

Recruitment ran from mid-January 2021 to November 2021. In total, 783 individuals registered for participation, of whom 602 were eligible and 421 signed informed consent (Fig. 1). Overall, 199 participants in the control arm and 162 in the intervention arm completed the baseline measurement and at least one follow-up measurement of the primary outcome, thereby reaching our target sample size of 360 participants [37]. Across trial arms, 73% of participants were female, 42% were highly educated and the average age was 58 (SD 11) years. The population characteristics were equally balanced between arms (Table 2) and across clusters [41], with the potential exception of prevalence of hyperlipidaemia.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Participant flowchart of the Supreme Nudge trial

Table 2 Population characteristics and baseline measurements in the Supreme Nudge trial (n = 361)

In addition, 233 of the participants also completed the baseline measurement and at least one follow-up measurement of the mobile coaching app intervention, of whom 114 were randomised to the mobile coaching app plus step counter app (intervention) and 119 only to the step counter app (control). Participant characteristics were equally balanced between groups and comparable to the total study population (Additional file 1: Table S4).

Primary outcome

On average over the 12-month intervention period (i.e. combining different time points), the nudging and pricing strategies did not affect diet quality (β − 1.1 (95% CI − 3.8 to 1.7)) compared to the control arm (Table 3). The ICCs for clustering of individual-level data and supermarket-level data were 0.39 and 0.04, respectively. Results were comparable when analysing diet quality at T1 (β − 0.8 (95% CI: − 3.7 to 2.2), n = 345) or T2 (β − 0.9 (95% CI: − 3.8 to 2.1), n = 357).

Table 3 Average changes in intervention participants over six to 12 months, compared to controls (ntotal = 361)

Secondary outcomes

There was no significant effect of nudging and pricing strategies on the total percentage of healthy purchasing over time (β 0.7 (95% CI: − 2.7 to 4.0)) nor on HbA1c (β 0.6 (95% CI: − 0.1 to 1.3)), lipid profile (LDL β − 0.0 (95% CI: − 0.2 to 0.1), HDL β − 0.0 (95% CI: − 0.1 to 0.0)), waist circumference (females β 0.7 (95% CI: − 0.8 to 2.2), males β 0.5 (95% CI: − 2.4 to 3.5)), the food-decision styles, and all social cognitive factors related to nudges (Table 3). The mobile coaching app intervention did not significantly change daily step count (β − 124.0 (95% CI: − 723.1 to 475.1), n = 233) compared to the control arm.

Participants in the intervention arm scored 0.3 points (95% CI: 0.1 to 0.5) higher on overall customer satisfaction, compared to the control arm (Table 3). After the trial, 39% of participants from intervention supermarkets indicated having noticed changes in the supermarket lay-out, compared to 24% of control participants (Additional file 1: Table S5).

Sensitivity analyses

Descriptive statistics of medication use per time point indicated no notable differences between arms over time (Additional file 1: Table S6). The descriptive statistics of the diet quality scores by food group and of the total percentages of healthier food purchasing from various food groups by time point suggested no underlying trends towards potential healthier consumption of different food groups or purchases (Additional file 1: Tables S7 and S8).

Age modified the association between the intervention and diet quality (βintervention group x age 0.16 (90% CI 0.01 to 0.31)). Nudging and pricing strategies resulted in higher diet quality with increasing age levels, as the oldest participants (80 years) in the intervention arm improved their diet quality with approximately 2 points more on average over time compared to those in the control arm (Additional file 2: Figure S2). Those youngest participants (30 years) reduced their diet quality with approximately 6 points on average over time compared to the control arm. Interaction terms with educational attainment and sex were not significant. For secondary outcomes, age modified the intervention effect on waist circumference among females (βintervention group x age − 0.17 (90% CI − 0.29 to − 0.05)), as nudging and pricing strategies resulted in a lower waist circumference at increasing age levels (Additional file 2: Figure S3). An interaction with age was found in similar direction for the total to HDL cholesterol ratio (βintervention group x age − 0.01 (90% CI − 0.03 to − 0.00)) (Additional file 2: Figure S4).

Supermarket-level results

Average pre-intervention sales of the total percentage of healthy products were 27.4% (SD 2.5) in control supermarkets and 27.5% (SD 2.1) in intervention supermarkets (Additional file 1: Table S9). Compared to control supermarkets and to pre-intervention sales trends, nudging and pricing strategies did not change the total percentage of healthy products sales (β − 0.0 (95% CI − 0.0 to 0.0)) nor the percentage of healthy products sales within various food groups (Table 4). Nudging and pricing strategies also did not significantly change retailer revenue (β − 76.9 Euros (95% CI − 274.6 to 120.4)). Results were comparable when excluding the intervention supermarket with the lowest implementation fidelity score over time (βtotal percentage of healthy sales 0.0 (95% CI − 0.0 to 0.1)).

Table 4 Average weekly changes in intervention supermarkets over six to 12 months, compared to controls (n = 12)

The median implementation fidelity score for all supermarkets over the intervention period was 3.6 (range 2.9–4.1) which translates to an implementation fidelity of 72%. Mean scores for individual supermarkets by time point are presented in Additional file 2: Figure S5.

Discussion

This cluster-RCT did not show effectiveness of a combination of real-world nudging and pricing strategies promoting healthier purchasing across a wide range of product groups on diet quality and food purchases nor on cardiometabolic risk markers. We found a small positive effect on customer satisfaction. Supermarket sales data trends were unaffected following implementation of nudging and pricing strategies, confirming the individual-level results. The mobile coaching app did not change daily step counts.

Strengths of this study include testing a combination of nudging and pricing strategies implemented across multiple food groups, the cluster-randomised and controlled design with long follow-up, the real-world nature of the data, outcome measurement of both diet quality and purchase data, and verification of individual-level results with supermarket-level results. Some limitations should be acknowledged. Quantitative questionnaires are prone to over- and underreporting and socially desirable answers. While the short 40-item FFQ minimised participant burden, it is less sensitive for detecting changes over time compared to a full-length FFQ. Although the co-creative intervention development and the supermarket-led implementation enabled a sustainable and scalable intervention, it probably also resulted in the interventions having less impact, with a stronger focus on nudging than on pricing strategies, and on promoting healthy versus discouraging unhealthy products. Despite recruiting in socially disadvantaged neighbourhoods, a relatively high proportion of participants had higher educational attainment and a relatively high diet quality score at baseline. However, potential attenuation of intervention effects due to selection bias is unlikely since the supermarket-level results, which reflect the intervention effects for all types of customers, confirmed the individual-level results. Last, since this trial was conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic, attenuation of potential intervention effects due to changed grocery shopping behaviours following the pandemic cannot be ruled out.

Thus far, only two comparable real-world studies have investigated a combination of nudging and pricing strategies, by targeting a specific food group. In contrast to our findings, these studies showed positive effects of combined placement, availability, and pricing interventions on peanuts sales at check-outs and on fruit and vegetable sales [33, 34]. Our null results might be explained by the relatively low intervention dosage of pricing strategies (targeting a mere 3% of the supermarket assortment according to a dynamic implementation pattern) and overall moderate implementation fidelity. The co-creation process showed that the supermarket chain was only willing to implement low risk interventions as more drastic changes (e.g. placing white bread behind a counter, or more extensive pricing strategies) were perceived to threaten profit margins and market position [43]. For pricing strategies to be effective, they may need to be structurally implemented within a food group (or multiple food groups) as a whole.

Other RCTs and non-randomised controlled trials, investigating nudges as single intervention components while using real-world purchasing, sales, or intake data, showed mixed effects. When significant, effect sizes were generally modest around 1–2% [64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74,75]. For pricing interventions, effects were more convincing [76,77,78,79,80,81]. More drastic nudge approaches would likely yield large(r) effect sizes (e.g. 2% increase in fruit and vegetable sales after relocating the fruits and vegetable section to a prominent location [75]) than, for example, an information nudge (e.g. 0.6% increase in healthy sales following shelf labels) [69]. Lessons from our and other real-world supermarket trials suggest that price discounts are effective and thus have the potential to improve diet quality, but only if applied structurally across enough food groups and when combined with price increases to maximise impact [76,77,78,79,80,81]. Nudging strategies are promising, but in real-world settings, they need to be drastic or multiple in order to sort a substantial effect [65, 66, 69,70,71,72,73,74,75]. In addition, nudging and pricing strategies may need to be combined to optimise their effectiveness [22, 33, 34]. Finally, our and other studies predominantly focused on stimulating healthier purchases, while mostly leaving unhealthy products untargeted. Considering that the majority of the supermarket assortment can be considered unhealthy [13], this leaves a large proportion of the supermarket assortment untargeted by interventions. Our findings in combination with the mixed evidence base on real-world studies suggests that unhealthy products also need to be targeted by interventions to exert an impact on diet quality.

The mobile coaching app did not change daily step counts. In a separate paper, we will present further exploratory findings suggesting effect modification by users’ perceived usefulness of the app. The lack of the main effect contrasts a meta-analysis of RCTs reporting a ~ 1500 step increase in daily step count with mobile interventions [32]. In our study, the app design did not allow for monitoring whether participants read the messages, but it might be that insufficient exposure to coaching messages led to the lack of effect on step counts [82]. Future studies should thus consider measuring user engagement.

Implications

Our results demonstrate that co-creation of interventional strategies with a retailer led to a predominant focus on the promotion of healthy products in supermarkets, which appeared not to be sufficient to shift purchasing patterns and dietary intake towards healthier patterns. Nevertheless, nudging and pricing strategies can be part of a comprehensive set of ambitious policy measures, while simultaneously focusing on discouraging unhealthier purchasing (e.g. restricting marketing and promotions of unhealthy products, banning of unhealthy products at checkouts, and restricting the availability of unhealthy products) [83, 84]. Such a combination of policy measures would create a level playing field among food retailers which is crucial in overcoming commercial barriers that impede impactful changes in supermarket environments. Furthermore, combined policy measures can lead to a small shift in population diet quality which can have a positive impact on population health outcomes [11]. Implementation fidelity and its effects on purchasing patterns needs to be monitored, and measures should be reformulated based on emerging insights.

There is a need for further studies focusing on the healthiness of purchase patterns or dietary intake patterns as a whole, rather than on single products or product groups, to better estimate the intervention implications for public health. Researchers may consider using more flexible study designs for real-world contexts that aim to generate public health benefits; various types of intervention components can be implemented simultaneously and innovated over a trial period based on real-time evaluations of their effectiveness [85]. Such an adaptive design could increase the chances of developing the most effective intervention(s), with the highest likelihood of achieving sustainable effects.

Conclusions

This study showed that co-created nudging and pricing strategies, as well as mobile physical activity coaching strategies, were not effective in a real-world context in promoting healthier diets and purchasing patterns, increasing step count or in improving cardiometabolic health. Focussing on the promotion of healthy products predominantly via nudging strategies targeting healthy products and some pricing strategies appears to be insufficient to counterbalance the high availability and promotion of unhealthy products in supermarkets. Comprehensive policy measures are essential to establish a level playing field among food retailers, focusing both on discouraging the purchase of unhealthy products and promoting healthier options. These policies should ensure fair competition among retailers while simultaneously working towards improving population diets and preventing cardiometabolic diseases.

Availability of data and materials

The data analysed during the current study are not publicly available as this would violate participant consent (individual-level data) or confidentiality agreements (supermarket-level data). The analyses plans and analytical codes are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ICC:

Intra-cluster correlation coefficient

HbA1c:

Glycated haemoglobin

HDL:

High-density lipoprotein cholesterol

LDL:

Low-density lipoprotein cholesterol

RCT:

Randomised controlled trial

SEP:

Socioeconomic position

References

  1. Roth GA, Mensah GA, Johnson CO, Addolorato G, Ammirati E, Baddour LM, et al. Global burden of cardiovascular diseases and risk factors, 1990–2019 update from the GBD 2019 study. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2020;76(25):2982–3021.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Safiri S, Karamzad N, Kaufman JS, Bell AW, Nejadghaderi SA, Sullman MJM, et al. Prevalence, deaths and disability-adjusted-life-years (DALYs) due to type 2 diabetes and its attributable risk factors in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: results from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Front Endocrinol. 2022;13:838027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Marmot, Michael & UCL Institute of Health Equity. Review of social determinants and the health divide in the WHO European Region: final report: World Health Organization. Regional Office for Europe; Updated reprint 2014 [Available from: https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/108636.

  4. Patino-Alonso MC, Recio-Rodriguez JI, Magdalena-Belio JF, Gine-Garriga M, Martinez-Vizcaino V, Fernandez-Alonso C, et al. Clustering of lifestyle characteristics and their association with cardio-metabolic health: the Lifestyles and Endothelial Dysfunction (EVIDENT) study. Brit J Nutr. 2015;114(6):943–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. WHO. Global status report on noncommunicable diseases 2014 Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2014 [Available from: https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/148114.

  6. Crouch R, Wilson A, Newbury J. A systematic review of the effectiveness of primary health education or intervention programs in improving rural women’s knowledge of heart disease risk factors and changing lifestyle behaviours. Int J Evid-Based Hea. 2011;9(3):236–45.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Middleton KR, Anton SD, Perri MG. Long-term adherence to health behavior change. Am J Lifestyle Med. 2013;7(6):395–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. McGill R, Anwar E, Orton L, Bromley H, Lloyd-Williams F, O’Flaherty M, et al. Are interventions to promote healthy eating equally effective for all? Systematic review of socioeconomic inequalities in impact. Bmc Public Health. 2015;15:457.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Lorenc T, Petticrew M, Welch V, Tugwell P. What types of interventions generate inequalities? Evidence from systematic reviews. J Epidemiol Commun H. 2013;67(2):190–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Adams J, Mytton O, White M, Monsivais P. Why are some population interventions for diet and obesity more equitable and effective than others? The role of individual agency. PLOS Medicine. 2016;13(4):e1001990.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Rose G. Sick individuals and sick populations. Int J Epidemiol. 1985;14(1):32–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Statista. Statista Global Consumer Survey (GCS): Grocery shopping by type in the Netherlands 2021: Statista; 2022 [Available from: https://www.statista.com/forecasts/1226870/grocery-shopping-by-type-in-the-netherlands.

  13. Poelman MP, Dijkstra SC, Djojosoeparto SK, de Vet EWML, Seidell JC, Kamphuis CBM. Monitoring van de mate van gezondheid van het aanbod en de promoties van supermarkten en out-of-home-ketens. Wageningen: Wageningen University & Research; 2021.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Thaler RH, Sunstein CR. Nudge: improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. New Haven: Yale University Press; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Hartmann-Boyce J, Bianchi F, Piernas C, Riches SP, Frie K, Nourse R, Jebb SA. Grocery store interventions to change food purchasing behaviors: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Am J Clin Nutr. 2018;107(6):1004–16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Shaw SC, Ntani G, Baird J, Vogel CA. A systematic review of the influences of food store product placement on dietary-related outcomes. Nutr Rev. 2020;78(12):1030–45.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Vecchio R, Cavallo C. Increasing healthy food choices through nudges: a systematic review. Food Quality and Preference. 2019;78:103714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Harbers MC, Beulens JWJ, Rutters F, de Boer F, Gillebaart M, Sluijs I, van der Schouw YT. The effects of nudges on purchases, food choice, and energy intake or content of purchases in real-life food purchasing environments: a systematic review and evidence synthesis. Nutr J. 2020;19(1):103.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Teng AM, Jones AC, Mizdrak A, Signal L, Genc M, Wilson N. Impact of sugar-sweetened beverage taxes on purchases and dietary intake: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev. 2019;20(9):1187–204.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Afshin A, Penalvo JL, Del Gobbo L, Silva J, Michaelson M, O’Flaherty M, et al. The prospective impact of food pricing on improving dietary consumption: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(3): e0172277.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. World Health Organization. Regional Office for Europe. WHO European Regional Obesity Report 2022. 2022 [Available from: https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/353747.

  22. Hoenink JC, Mackenbach JD, Waterlander W, Lakerveld J, van der Laan N, Beulens JWJ. The effects of nudging and pricing on healthy food purchasing behavior in a virtual supermarket setting: a randomized experiment. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2020;17(1):98.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Forberger S, Reisch L, Kampfmann T, Zeeb H. Nuding to move: a scoping review of the use of choice architecture interventions to promote physical activity in the general population. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2019;16(77):1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Kremers SP, Eves FF, Andersen RE. Environmental changes to promote physical activity and healthy dietary behavior. J Environ Public Health. 2012;2012: 470858.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Ernsting C, Dombrowski SU, Oedekoven M, Sullivan JLO, Kanzler M, Kuhlmey A, Gellert P. Using smartphones and health apps to change and manage health behaviors: a population-based survey. J Med Internet Res. 2017;19(4):e101.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Cho J, Park D, Lee HE. Cognitive factors of using health apps: systematic analysis of relationships among health consciousness, health information orientation, eHealth literacy, and health app use efficacy. J Med Internet Res. 2014;16(5):206–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Carroll JK, Moorhead A, Bond R, LeBlanc WG, Petrella RJ, Fiscella K. Who uses mobile phone health apps and does use matter? A secondary data analytics approach. J Med Internet Res. 2017;19(4): e125.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Yerrakalva D, Yerrakalva D, Hajna S, Griffin S. Effects of mobile health app interventions on sedentary time, physical activity, and fitness in older adults: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Med Internet Res. 2019;21(11): e14343.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Gardner B, de Bruijn GJ, Lally P. A systematic review and meta-analysis of applications of the self-report habit index to nutrition and physical activity behaviours. Ann Behav Med. 2011;42(2):174–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Gal R, May AM, van Overmeeren EJ, Simons M, Monninkhof EM. The effect of physical activity interventions comprising wearables and smartphone applications on physical activity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med Open. 2018;4(1):42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Fanning J, Mullen SP, McAuley E. Increasing physical activity with mobile devices: a meta-analysis. J Med Internet Res. 2012;14(6):159–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Monninghoff A, Kramer JN, Hess AJ, Ismailova K, Teepe GW, Car LT, et al. Long-term effectiveness of mhealth physical activity interventions: systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Med Internet Res. 2021;23(4):e26699.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Williams MB, Wang WY, Taniguchi T, Salvatore AL, Groover WK, Wetherill M, et al. Impact of a healthy retail intervention on fruits and vegetables and total sales in tribally owned convenience stores: findings from the THRIVE study. Health Promot Pract. 2021;22(6):796–805.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Chapman LE, Burstein S, Sadeghzadeh C, Sheppard B, De Marco M. Evaluation of a healthy checkout lane “nudge” on grocery and convenience store sales of a price-promoted nutritious food. Health Promot Pract. 2023;24(1):111–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Lovhaug AL, Granheim SI, Djojosoeparto SK, Harrington JM, Kamphuis CBM, Poelman MP, et al. The potential of food environment policies to reduce socioeconomic inequalities in diets and to improve healthy diets among lower socioeconomic groups: an umbrella review. Bmc Public Health. 2022;22(1):433.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Lakerveld J, Mackenbach JD, de Boer F, Brandhorst B, Broerse JEW, de Bruijn GJ, et al. Improving cardiometabolic health through nudging dietary behaviours and physical activity in low SES adults: design of the Supreme Nudge project. Bmc Public Health. 2018;18:899.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Stuber JM, Mackenbach JD, de Boer FE, de Bruijn GJ, Gillebaart M, Harbers MC, et al. Reducing cardiometabolic risk in adults with a low socioeconomic position: protocol of the Supreme Nudge parallel cluster-randomised controlled supermarket trial. Nutr J. 2020;19(1):46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Stuber JM, Mackenbach JD, de Boer FE, de Bruijn GJ, Gillebaart M, Harbers MC, et al. Correction: Reducing cardiometabolic risk in adults with a low socioeconomic position: protocol of the Supreme Nudge parallel cluster-randomised controlled supermarket trial. Nutr J. 2022;21(1):44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Campbell MK, Piaggio G, Elbourne DR, Altman DG, Group C. Consort 2010 statement: extension to cluster randomised trials. BMJ. 2012;345:e5661.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. SCP. The Netherlands Institute for Social Research. Status scores 2018, vol. 2019. Den Haag: The Netherlands Institute for Social Research; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Stuber JM, Hoek BACE, Vos AL, Smit EG, Lakerveld J, Mackenbach JD, Beulens JWJ. Participant recruitment, baseline characteristics and at-home-measurements of cardiometabolic risk markers: insights from the Supreme Nudge parallel cluster-randomised controlled supermarket trial. Trials. 2023;24(159):1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Kromhout D, Spaaij CJK, de Goede J, Weggemans RM, Brug J, Geleijnse JM, et al. The 2015 Dutch food-based dietary guidelines. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2016;70(8):869–78.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Middel CNH, Schuitmaker-Warnaar TJ, Mackenbach JD, Broerse JEW. Designing a healthy food-store intervention; a co-creative process between interventionists and supermarket actors. Int J Health Policy Manag. 2022;11(10):2175–88.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Brink E, van Rossum C, Postma-Smeets A, Stafleu A, Wolvers D, van Dooren C, et al. Development of healthy and sustainable food-based dietary guidelines for the Netherlands. Public Health Nutr. 2019;22(13):2419–35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Hollands GJ, Bignardi G, Johnston M, Kelly MP, Ogilvie D, Petticrew M, et al. The TIPPME intervention typology for changing environments to change behaviour. Nat Hum Behav. 2017;1(8):0140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Vos AL, de Bruijn GJ, Klein MCA, Lakerveld J, Boerman SC, Smit EG. SNapp, a tailored smartphone app intervention to promote walking in adults of low socioeconomic position: development and qualitative pilot study. JMIR Form Res. 2023;7:e40851.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Looman M, Feskens EJM, de Rijk M, Meijboom S, Biesbroek S, Temme EHM, et al. Development and evaluation of the Dutch Healthy Diet index 2015. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20(13):2289–99.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. van Lee L, Feskens EJ, Meijboom S, Hooft van Huysduynen EJ, van’t Veer P, de Vries JH, Geelen A. Evaluation of a screener to assess diet quality in the Netherlands. Br J Nutr. 2016;115(3):517–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Looman M, Feskens EJ, de Rijk M, Meijboom S, Biesbroek S, Temme EH, et al. Development and evaluation of the Dutch Healthy Diet index 2015. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20(13):2289–99.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Verplanken B, Herabadi A. Individual differences in impulse buying tendency: feeling and no thinking. Eur J Personality. 2001;15:S71–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Spinella M. Normative data and a short form of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Int J Neurosci. 2007;117(3):359–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Scott SG, Bruce RA. Decision-Making Style - the Development and Assessment of a New Measure. Educ Psychol Meas. 1995;55(5):818–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Verplanken B, Orbell S. Reflections on past behavior: a self-report index of habit strength. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2003;33(6):1313–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Ersche KD, Lim TV, Ward LHE, Robbins TW, Stochl J. Creature of Habit: a self-report measure of habitual routines and automatic tendencies in everyday life. Pers Indiv Differ. 2017;116:73–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. de Ridder D, Gillebaart M. What’s in a nudge. Tijdschrift voor gezondheidswetenschappen. 2016;94(7):261–5.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Forwood SE, Ahern AL, Hollands GJ, Ng YL, Marteau TM. Priming healthy eating. You can’t prime all the people all of the time. Appetite. 2015;89:93–102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. GiskeS K, Van Lenthe FJ, Brug J, Mackenbach JP, Turrell G. Socioeconomic inequalities in food purchasing: the contribution of respondent-perceived and actual (objectively measured) price and availability of foods. Prev Med. 2007;45(1):41–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. van Ansem WJC, Schrijvers CTM, Rodenburg G, van de Mheen D. Is there an association between the home food environment, the local food shopping environment and children’s fruit and vegetable intake? Results from the Dutch INPACT study. Public Health Nutr. 2013;16(7):1206–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Dijkstra SC, Neter JE, van Stralen MM, Knol DL, Brouwer IA, Huisman M, Visser M. The role of perceived barriers in explaining socio-economic status differences in adherence to the fruit, vegetable and fish guidelines in older adults: a mediation study. Public Health Nutr. 2015;18(5):797–808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Higgs S, Liu J, Collins EIM, Thomas JM. Using social norms to encourage healthier eating. Nutr Bull. 2019;44(1):43–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Stuber JM, Mackenbach JD, Lakerveld J, Beulens JWJ. Pre-registration: Controlled interrupted time series analysis of store-level sales data as part of the Supreme Nudge randomised controlled supermarket trial: OSF Preregistration; 2022 [Available from: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/6KTVJ.

  62. Bernal JL, Cummins S, Gasparrini A. The use of controls in interrupted time series studies of public health interventions. Int J Epidemiol. 2018;47(6):2082–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Bernal JL, Cummins S, Gasparrini A. Interrupted time series regression for the evaluation of public health interventions: a tutorial. Int J Epidemiol. 2017;46(1):348–55.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Ayala GX, Pickrel JL, Baquero B, Sanchez-Flack J, Lin SF, Belch G, et al. The El Valor de Nuestra Salud clustered randomized controlled trial store-based intervention to promote fruit and vegetable purchasing and consumption. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2022;19(1):19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Foster GD, Karpyn A, Wojtanowski AC, Davis E, Weiss S, Brensinger C, et al. Placement and promotion strategies to increase sales of healthier products in supermarkets in low-income, ethnically diverse neighborhoods: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;99(6):1359–68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Budd N, Jeffries JK, Jones-Smith J, Kharmats A, McDermott AY, Gittelsohn J. Store-directed price promotions and communications strategies improve healthier food supply and demand: impact results from a randomized controlled, Baltimore City store-intervention trial. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20(18):3349–59.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Huitink M, Poelman MP, Seidell JC, Pleus M, Hofkamp T, Kuin C, Dijkstra SC. Can unhealthy food purchases at checkout counters be discouraged by introducing healthier snacks? A real-life experiment in supermarkets in deprived urban areas in the Netherlands. BMC Public Health. 2020;20(1):542.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Winkler LL, Christensen U, Glumer C, Bloch P, Mikkelsen BE, Wansink B, Toft U. Substituting sugar confectionery with fruit and healthy snacks at checkout - a win-win strategy for consumers and food stores? a study on consumer attitudes and sales effects of a healthy supermarket intervention. Bmc Public Health. 2016;16:1184.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Cameron AJ, Brown A, Orellana L, Marshall J, Charlton E, Ngan WW, et al. Change in the healthiness of foods sold in an australian supermarket chain following implementation of a shelf tag intervention based on the health star rating system. Nutrients. 2022;14(12):2394.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Brimblecombe J, McMahon E, Ferguson M, De Silva K, Peeters A, Miles E, et al. Effect of restricted retail merchandising of discretionary food and beverages on population diet: a pragmatic randomised controlled trial. Lancet Planet Health. 2020;4(10):e463–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Piernas C, Cook B, Stevens R, Stewart C, Hollowell J, Scarborough P, Jebb SA. Estimating the effect of moving meat-free products to the meat aisle on sales of meat and meat-free products: a non-randomised controlled intervention study in a large UK supermarket chain. Plos Medicine. 2021;18(7):e1003715.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Gamburzew A, Darcel N, Gazan R, Dubois C, Maillot M, Tome D, et al. In-store marketing of inexpensive foods with good nutritional quality in disadvantaged neighborhoods: increased awareness, understanding, and purchasing. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2016;13:104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Huitink M, Poelman MP, van den Eynde E, Seidell JC, Dijkstra SC. Social norm nudges in shopping trolleys to promote vegetable purchases: a quasi-experimental study in a supermarket in a deprived urban area in the Netherlands. Appetite. 2020;151:104655.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Song HJ, Gittelsohn J, Kim M, Suratkar S, Sharma S, Anliker J. A corner store intervention in a low-income urban community is associated with increased availability and sales of some healthy foods. Public Health Nutr. 2009;12(11):2060–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Vogel C, Crozier S, Penn-Newman D, Ball K, Moon G, Lord J, et al. Altering product placement to create a healthier layout in supermarkets: Outcomes on store sales, customer purchasing, and diet in a prospective matched controlled cluster study. PLoS Med. 2021;18(9):e1003729.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Ball K, McNaughton SA, Le HN, Gold L, Ni Mhurchu C, Abbott G, et al. Influence of price discounts and skill-building strategies on purchase and consumption of healthy food and beverages: outcomes of the Supermarket Healthy Eating for Life randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2015;101(5):1055–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Bernales-Korins M, Ang IYH, Khan S, Geliebter A. Psychosocial influences on fruit and vegetable intake following a NYC supermarket discount. Obesity. 2017;25(8):1321–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Moran A, Thorndike A, Franckle R, Boulos R, Doran H, Fulay A, et al. Financial incentives increase purchases of fruit and vegetables among lower-income households with children. Health Affair. 2019;38(9):1557–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Ni Mhurchu C, Blakely T, Jiang Y, Eyles HC, Rodgers A. Effects of price discounts and tailored nutrition education on supermarket purchases: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;91(3):736–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Polacsek M, Moran A, Thorndike AN, Boulos R, Franckle RL, Greene JC, et al. A Supermarket double-dollar incentive program increases purchases of fresh fruits and vegetables among low-income families with children: the Healthy Double study. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2018;50(3):217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Waterlander WE, de Boer MR, Schuit AJ, Seidell JC, Steenhuis IHM. Price discounts significantly enhance fruit and vegetable purchases when combined with nutrition education: a randomized controlled supermarket trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;97(4):886–95.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Schoeppe S, Alley S, Van Lippevelde W, Bray NA, Williams SL, Duncan MJ, Vandelanotte C. Efficacy of interventions that use apps to improve diet, physical activity and sedentary behaviour: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2016;13:137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. WHO. European Regional Obesity Report 2022. Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe; 2022.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Jacobson MF, Krieger J, Brownell KD. Potential policy approaches to address diet-related diseases. JAMA. 2018;320(4):341–2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Loorbach DA. Transition Management: new mode of governance for sustainable development 2007 [Available from: http://hdl.handle.net/1765/10200.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the supermarket chain for enabling real-world intervention implementation and for sharing sales data, and we thank all study participants for their efforts. In addition, we thank the scientific advisory board of the Supreme Nudge consortium for their valuable input and advises during the trial conception phase and its execution: M. Droomers, G. J. Hollands, M. Mazzocchi, P. Monsivais, D. O'Gorman, and C. D. A. Stehouwer.

Funding

This study was part of the ‘Sustainable Prevention of Cardiometabolic Risk through Nudging Health Behaviours’ (Supreme Nudge) project. The Supreme Nudge project is funded by the Dutch Heart Foundation (grant number CVON2016-04) and the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMw) (531003001). Funders had no role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript nor do they have authority on the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Consortia

Contributions

JWJB, JDM, and JL were initiators of the Supreme Nudge trial and, together with JMS, leading contributors to its conceptual design and development of the trial protocol. GJB, MG, JCH, CNHM, DTDR, YTS, EGS, EV, ALV, and WEW contributed to the development of the protocol. JCH, CNHM, and ALV contributed to the trial conduct and data collection. JMS coordinated the data collection, performed data cleaning and analyses of all data (except the step count data), and drafted the initial manuscript. ALV conducted the data collection, data cleaning, and analysis of the step count data. JWJB, JDM, and JL supervised the project. All authors provided detailed commentaries to the initial manuscript draft and approved the final version, and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Authors’ Twitter handles

Josine Stuber: @JosineStuber; Joreintje Mackenbach: @JoreintjeM; Jody Hoenink: @JHoenink; Liesbeth Velema: @liesbeth_velema; Jeroen Lakerveld: @jlakerveld; Wilma Waterlander: @wwaterlander.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Josine M. Stuber.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was performed in line with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. The study protocol was approved by the Medical Ethics Review Committee of VU University Medical Center in Amsterdam, the Netherlands (reference number: 2019.334). All participants provided written informed consent prior to study enrolment.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Tables S1-S9.

Table S1—Available number and type of healthy and unhealthy products per food group on average across participating supermarkets (n = 12); Table S2—Intervention dosage of pricing strategies across food groups over the trial period; Table S3—Individual-level outcomes and their measurement methods; Table S4—Population characteristics and baseline measurements of participants of the mobile coaching app intervention in the Supreme Nudge trial (n = 233), stratified by mobile coaching app arm; Table S5—Descriptive statistics of nudge appreciation and nudge awareness during the last follow-up measurement at month 6 or month 12 (n = 332); Table S6—Descriptive statistics of medication use per time point (n = 361), stratified by trial arm; Table S7—Descriptive statistics per time point of the diet quality scores per food group (n = 361), stratified by trial arm; Table S8—Descriptive statistics per time point of the total percentage of healthy purchasing from different food groups (n = 217), stratified by trial arm; Table S9—Average pre-intervention and post-intervention sales per week for all intervention and all control supermarkets for each supermarket-level outcome (n = 12).

Additional file 2: Figures S1-S5.

Figures S1a-k—Photo examples of nudge intervention: Figure S2—Average diet quality scores over time for different age levels in the intervention group and the control group of the Supreme Nudge trial (n = 361); Figure S3—Average waist circumference among females over time for different age levels in the intervention group and the control group of the Supreme Nudge trial (n = 239); Figure S4—Average total cholesterol to HDL ratio over time for different age levels in the intervention group and the control group of the Supreme Nudge trial (n = 290); Figure S5—Mean implementation fidelity scores (range 1–5) per month over the intervention period of one year, by supermarket.

Additional file 3.

CONSORT 2010 checklist for cluster randomised trials.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stuber, J.M., Mackenbach, J.D., de Bruijn, GJ. et al. Real-world nudging, pricing, and mobile physical activity coaching was insufficient to improve lifestyle behaviours and cardiometabolic health: the Supreme Nudge parallel cluster-randomised controlled supermarket trial. BMC Med 22, 52 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-024-03268-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-024-03268-4

Keywords